2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104171
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How emotion is learned: Semantic learning of novel words in emotional contexts

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

6
19
1

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 98 publications
6
19
1
Order By: Relevance
“…With respect to the second research question, the results of the mixed-effects modeling on the learning outcomes for emotion-laden words revealed that participants in the current study were predicted to recall and retain neutral words better than either positive-or negative-laden items. While this generally supports prior studies in psychology, both with monolingual (e.g., Altarriba & Bauer, 2004;Jay et al, 2008;Snefjella et al, 2020) and bilingual (e.g., Ayçiçegi & Harris, 2004;Ayçiçegi-Dinn & Caldwell-Harris, 2009;Ferré et al, 2010) participants, that mark a contrast between emotion-laden and neutral words, the current study contradicts previous research in psycholinguistics (e.g., Altarriba & Bauer, 2004;Ayçiçegi & Harris, 2004;El-Dakhs & Altarriba, 2019;Kazanas & Altarriba, 2015a, 2015bSnefjella et al, 2020), neurolinguistics (e.g., Hinojosa et al, 2020), and psychology (e.g., Hadley & MacKay, 2006;Jay et al, 2008;MacKay & Ahmetzanov, 2005;MacKay et al, 2004) on learning and memory enhancement. In this research, findings largely show that emotion (e.g., love, hate), emotion-laden (e.g., puppy, jail), and taboo words (e.g., reproductive organs, expletives) are recalled more successfully than neutral words.…”
Section: Learning Expectations For Emotion-laden Vocabularysupporting
confidence: 89%
“…With respect to the second research question, the results of the mixed-effects modeling on the learning outcomes for emotion-laden words revealed that participants in the current study were predicted to recall and retain neutral words better than either positive-or negative-laden items. While this generally supports prior studies in psychology, both with monolingual (e.g., Altarriba & Bauer, 2004;Jay et al, 2008;Snefjella et al, 2020) and bilingual (e.g., Ayçiçegi & Harris, 2004;Ayçiçegi-Dinn & Caldwell-Harris, 2009;Ferré et al, 2010) participants, that mark a contrast between emotion-laden and neutral words, the current study contradicts previous research in psycholinguistics (e.g., Altarriba & Bauer, 2004;Ayçiçegi & Harris, 2004;El-Dakhs & Altarriba, 2019;Kazanas & Altarriba, 2015a, 2015bSnefjella et al, 2020), neurolinguistics (e.g., Hinojosa et al, 2020), and psychology (e.g., Hadley & MacKay, 2006;Jay et al, 2008;MacKay & Ahmetzanov, 2005;MacKay et al, 2004) on learning and memory enhancement. In this research, findings largely show that emotion (e.g., love, hate), emotion-laden (e.g., puppy, jail), and taboo words (e.g., reproductive organs, expletives) are recalled more successfully than neutral words.…”
Section: Learning Expectations For Emotion-laden Vocabularysupporting
confidence: 89%
“…The findings reviewed above are consistent with the suggestion that distributional models that take only language data as input may be adequate for broadly capturing semantic similarity, and emerging evidence suggests that novel words may, to some degree, acquire embodied representations by virtue of the embodied properties of the contexts in which they appear (Günther et al, 2020;Snefjella et al, 2020; see also Snefjella & Kuperman, 2016). An interesting open question is whether embodied experience alone (e.g., Öttl et al, 2017) also facilitates category learning, or whether language is necessary for carving categorical boundaries into our experience (for review, see Lupyan, 2012).…”
Section: Effects Of Embodied and Distributional Linguistic Informatiosupporting
confidence: 75%
“…Specifically, after novel words were learned in contexts implying upwards or downwards movements, action-congruency effects were found if participants had to access word meaning. This suggests that embodied representations can be acquired via contextual association (see also Snefjella et al, 2020;Snefjella & Kuperman, 2016). However, when people learned similar concepts but were tested for action-congruency effects using a task that did not require accessing the words' meanings (Günther et al, 2018) no action-congruency effects were observed.…”
Section: Effects Of Embodied and Distributional Linguistic Informatiomentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The findings reviewed above are consistent with the suggestion that distributional models that take only language data as input may be adequate for broadly capturing semantic similarity, and emerging evidence suggests that novel words may, to some degree, acquire embodied representations by virtue of the embodied properties of the contexts in which they appear (Günther et al, 2020;Snefjella et al, 2020; see also Snefjella & Kuperman, 2016). An interesting open question is whether embodied experience alone (e.g., Öttl et al, 2017) also facilitates category learning, or whether language is necessary for carving categorical boundaries into our experience (for review, see Lupyan, 2012).…”
Section: Effects Of Embodied and Distributional Linguistic Information On Semantic Processingsupporting
confidence: 75%