“…Contrast sensitivity, the ability to discriminate visual patterns from a uniform background, is a fundamental measure of visual function that depends substantially on the pattern's spatial frequency (SF) and eccentricity ( Campbell & Robson, 1968 ; De Valois, Morgan, Snodderly, 1974 ; Hilz & Cavonius, 1974 ; Kelly, 1977 ; Owsley, 2003 ; Robson, 1966 ; Robson & Graham, 1981 ; Rovamo Virsu, & Näsänen, 1978 ; Virsu & Rovamo, 1979 ). Covert spatial attention (henceforth attention)—the prioritization of discrete spatial locations in the absence of eye movements—enhances contrast sensitivity for a wide range of spatial frequencies and at several eccentricities ( Barbot, Landy, & Carrasco, 2011 , Barbot, Landy, & Carrasco, 2012 ; Cameron, Tai, & Carrasco, 2002 ; Carrasco, Penpeci-Talgar, & Eckstein, 2000 ; Carrasco & McElree, 2001 ; Dosher & Lu, 2000a ; Dosher & Lu, 2000b ; Fernández et al, 2019 ; Foley & Schwarz, 1998 ; Herrmann, Montaser-Kouhsari, Carrasco, & Heeger, 2010 ; Huang & Dobkins, 2005 ; Lee et al, 1997 ; Lee, Itti, Koch, & Braun, 1999 ; Ling & Carrasco, 2006a ; Ling & Carrasco, 2006b ; Liu, Pestilli, & Carrasco, 2005 ; Lu, Lesmes, & Dosher, 2002 ; Lu & Dosher, 1998 ; Lu & Dosher, 2000 ; Morgan, Ward, & Castet, et al,1998 ; Morrone, Denti, & Spinelli, 2002 ; Morrone, Denti, & Spinelli, 2004 ; Pestilli, Viera, & Carrasco, 2007 ; Pestilli & Carrasco, 2005 ; Smith, Wolfgang, & Sinclair, 2004 ; Solomon, 2004 ; Solomon, Lavie, & Morgan, 1997 ; for reviews see, Carrasco, 2006 , Carrasco, 2011 ; Carrasco, 2014 ). However, very few studies have directly compared the effects of exogenous (involuntary) and endogenous (voluntary) attention on contrast sensitivity ( Barbot et al, 2012 ; Herrmann et al, 2010 ; Ling & Carrasco, 2006b ; Lu & Dosher, 2000 ), and no single study has jointly man...…”