2013
DOI: 10.1002/wea.2094
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How forecasts expressing uncertainty are perceived by UK students

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

11
38
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
11
38
1
Order By: Relevance
“…To compare the understanding of probabilistic forecast information for this cohort with previous groups, respondents were asked a standard question (about their interpretation of probabilistic information) that was used by several previous studies (Gigerenzer et al , ; Morss et al , ; Peachey et al , ):
Imagine that the weather forecast predicts ‘There is a 30% chance of rain tomorrow’. Please indicate which of the following is the most appropriate interpretation of the forecast?
…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 78%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To compare the understanding of probabilistic forecast information for this cohort with previous groups, respondents were asked a standard question (about their interpretation of probabilistic information) that was used by several previous studies (Gigerenzer et al , ; Morss et al , ; Peachey et al , ):
Imagine that the weather forecast predicts ‘There is a 30% chance of rain tomorrow’. Please indicate which of the following is the most appropriate interpretation of the forecast?
…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 78%
“…In common with previous studies, a majority of respondents did not interpret this statement correctly, and a substantial fraction answered ‘other’ (again in common with Morss et al , ; Joslyn et al , ; Peachey et al , ) which suggests widespread difficulty in interpreting probabilistic forecast statements. In this sample, interestingly, of the three categories (region, time and days) in which respondents could indicate that they understood what the statement meant, the correct interpretation (days) was the most common answer (27%) indicating some understanding of PoP forecasts.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies (Morss et al ., , ; Lazo et al ., ; Joslyn and Savelli, ; Savelli and Joslyn, ; Peachey et al ., ) show that most people are inclined to assign a certain range of uncertainty to a single‐value forecast provided by a deterministic model: uncertainty is anticipated by the user even when such information is not intentionally supplied.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, previous studies found that probabilities of precipitation gave rise to divergent interpretations by various members of the public (e.g., Gigerenzer et al 2005). In addition, there is no consensus as to which format should be used to present forecast uncertainty (e.g., probabilities, frequencies, odds, or expected ranges) among users (Peachey et al 2013) or scientists (LeClerc and Joslyn 2012). Another challenge occurred when all the provided information was not used, such as when informants looked at only the cloud symbols in the diagram and not at the hatched precipitation columns.…”
Section: Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent studies have focused on the communication of uncertainty information in weather reports (Gigerenzer et al 2005;Roulston et al 2006;Morss et al 2008;, Joslyn et al 2009Joslyn and Savelli 2010;Peachey et al 2013). However, these (mainly quantitative) studies are primarily concerned with interpretations of one type of uncertainty information: the probability of precipitation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%