2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104641
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How peer influence shapes value computation in moral decision-making

Abstract: Moral behavior is susceptible to peer influence. How does information from peers influence moral preferences? We used drift-diffusion modeling to show that peer influence changes the value of moral behavior by prioritizing the choice attributes that align with peers' goals. Study 1 ( N = 100; preregistered) showed that participants accurately inferred the goals of prosocial and antisocial peers when observing their moral decisions. In Study 2 ( N = 68), participant… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

3
50
0
1

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 77 publications
3
50
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Such a model, however, requires either a prohibitive number of trials per participant, or the integration of some other type of information. This problem could possibly be overcome by integrating temporal information to simple choice data: several studies have successfully analysed subjective choices with this method before using so-called sequential sampling models (SSM, see for instance [ 21 , 89 ]). While this approach would require challenging improvements, such as disentangling variability both within and between trials, it could also promote the analysis of other decision components, such as the trade-off between fidelity with one’s preferences and speed in making a decision.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Such a model, however, requires either a prohibitive number of trials per participant, or the integration of some other type of information. This problem could possibly be overcome by integrating temporal information to simple choice data: several studies have successfully analysed subjective choices with this method before using so-called sequential sampling models (SSM, see for instance [ 21 , 89 ]). While this approach would require challenging improvements, such as disentangling variability both within and between trials, it could also promote the analysis of other decision components, such as the trade-off between fidelity with one’s preferences and speed in making a decision.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This means that we may be missing a connection between how close one’s initial attitude is to the observed agent’s and how much she will conform after learning. Indeed, recent research suggests that similarity with the observed agent influences the effect of conformity [ 21 ]. To solve this problem, in future experiments we propose to dynamically adjust the attitude of the agent depending on participants’ own attitude.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Human values can and do vary: throughout history (Elias, 1978;Inglehart, 2008;Morris, 2015;Pinker, 2012), throughout the individual's development (House et al, 2013), across populations, cultures, ecologies, moral communities, and individuals (Gelfand et al, 2011;Hofstede, 2001;Inglehart, 2008;Singer, 2011), and across situations within individuals (Aarøe & Petersen, 2014;DeScioli et al, 2014). This suggests that values are sometimes computed to match not an objective standard (e.g., the protein content of food) but a local, possibly variable social consensus-the values that fellow group members are inferred to hold (Yu et al, 2021; see also Kuran, 1997;Zentall & Galef, 1988;Rendell et al, 2010). However, note that socially contingent values appear to reflect variation in how the open parameters of the valuation architecture are filled in from one individual or community or era to the next, rather than variation in the valuation architecture itself.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%