2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How we categorize objects is related to how we remember them: The shape bias as a memory bias

Abstract: The “shape bias” describes the phenomenon that, after a certain point in development, children and adults generalize object categories based upon shape to a greater degree than other perceptual features. The focus of research on the shape bias has been to examine the types of information that learners attend to in one moment in time. The current work takes a different approach by examining whether learners' categorical biases are related to their retention of information across time. In three experiments, chil… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
10
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
1
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Conversely, our study with US adults was conducted online, and this method may have elicited a stronger shape bias, possibly due to reduced salience in materials, and increased variability in color across participants' monitors. However, it is useful to note that multiple studies have established a shape bias in US adults in a variety of methods that include interaction with real objects (Li, et al, 2009) and presentations on screens (Vlach, 2016). Therefore, while our results are consistent with previous work documenting a shape bias in US adults, the comparison between US and Tsimane' participants should be interpreted cautiously, as it is possible that seeing real-world objects leads to different generalization biases than seeing objects on a screen.…”
Section: Study Limitationssupporting
confidence: 88%
“…Conversely, our study with US adults was conducted online, and this method may have elicited a stronger shape bias, possibly due to reduced salience in materials, and increased variability in color across participants' monitors. However, it is useful to note that multiple studies have established a shape bias in US adults in a variety of methods that include interaction with real objects (Li, et al, 2009) and presentations on screens (Vlach, 2016). Therefore, while our results are consistent with previous work documenting a shape bias in US adults, the comparison between US and Tsimane' participants should be interpreted cautiously, as it is possible that seeing real-world objects leads to different generalization biases than seeing objects on a screen.…”
Section: Study Limitationssupporting
confidence: 88%
“…One potential trigger for this shift to classification is the onset of language learning. Indeed, as children begin to verbally identify objects they are increasingly better at encoding objects' features (Vlach, 2016;Westermann & Mareschal, 2014), suggesting that the development of language may contribute to a focus on identification (Smith, Jones, & Landau, 1996;Xu & Carey, 1996;Xu et al, 1999). Interestingly, mature nonverbal primates (e.g., gorillas, orangutans, and chimpanzees), who presumably have mastered object individuation, continue to favor spatial information over features (Haun et al, 2006), lending further support to the hypothesis that language may give rise to the shift in focus on classification.…”
Section: Development Of Individuation and Classificationmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…The early slow learning about a few objects and their names before first birthday may be Lesson 1. The visual pervasiveness of a select set of objects in a cluttered visual field may build strong visual memories for these few things enabling infants them to remember seen things and their heard names [44, 51]. Lesson 2 may use very different training data.…”
Section: Infants Create a Curriculum For Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%