2016
DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.12617
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Hypervigilance‐avoidance in children with anxiety disorders: magnetoencephalographic evidence

Abstract: These results support the hypothesis of early sensory hypervigilance followed by later avoidance of threat in anxiety disordered children, presumably modulated by early reduced and later enhanced prefrontal inhibition. This neuronal hypervigilance-avoidance pattern unfolds gradually with increasing trait anxiety, reflecting a progressively biased allocation of attention to threat.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, high trait anxious participants may have tried to avoid and disengage in stimulus processing during conditions allowing them to focus on non‐facial features. This finding can be related to the vigilance‐avoidance hypothesis which implies an initial vigilance toward aversive stimuli, followed by avoidance of threatening information (Mathews, 1990; Wessing et al., 2017). Previous studies suggest that avoidance of threatening information is a core deficit in anxiety, explained by inhibition mechanisms of later processing stages (Foa & Kozak, 1986; Mogg et al., 2004).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…Therefore, high trait anxious participants may have tried to avoid and disengage in stimulus processing during conditions allowing them to focus on non‐facial features. This finding can be related to the vigilance‐avoidance hypothesis which implies an initial vigilance toward aversive stimuli, followed by avoidance of threatening information (Mathews, 1990; Wessing et al., 2017). Previous studies suggest that avoidance of threatening information is a core deficit in anxiety, explained by inhibition mechanisms of later processing stages (Foa & Kozak, 1986; Mogg et al., 2004).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…However, as Liddell et al (2004) did not employ source reconstruction approaches, the neuronal generators were not revealed in this particular study and thus remained speculative. Yet, there is ample evidence that distributed neuronal sources in visual processing areas and frontal regions contribute to the LPP ( Olofsson et al, 2008 ; Wessing et al, 2016 ). Several authors have argued that stronger late-latency brain activation in visual processing areas reflects an ongoing, increasingly elaborate perceptual evaluation of emotional stimuli based on reentrant processing ( Schupp et al, 2004a ; Wessing et al, 2016 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet, there is ample evidence that distributed neuronal sources in visual processing areas and frontal regions contribute to the LPP ( Olofsson et al, 2008 ; Wessing et al, 2016 ). Several authors have argued that stronger late-latency brain activation in visual processing areas reflects an ongoing, increasingly elaborate perceptual evaluation of emotional stimuli based on reentrant processing ( Schupp et al, 2004a ; Wessing et al, 2016 ). The strength of this effect can be modulated by several factors including the use of voluntary regulatory strategies (e.g., reappraisal, Hajcak and Nieuwenhuis, 2006 ), as well as rDLPFC inhibition and excitation ( Notzon et al, 2017 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations