2015
DOI: 10.1007/s10339-015-0655-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

I believe I’m good at orienting myself… But is that true?

Abstract: The present study aimed to analyse beliefs that men and women have with respect to their sense of direction (SOD) and whether they correlate with spatial environmental task performance. Eighty-four students filled in the short version of the Familiarity and Spatial Cognitive Style Scale to evaluate beliefs on their SOD, knowledge of the city (TK), spatial ability (SA) and wayfinding (WA) and performed three spatial environmental tasks. Results showed that gender did not predict the performance on the spatial e… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
20
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
(48 reference statements)
3
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These strategies are named “spatial styles.” Some studies have found that women are usually prone to use “landmark” or “route styles,” where egocentric information is necessary. On the contrary, men usually choose a “survey style,” which implies allocentric information and capacity to represent the environment as a map (Coluccia and Louse, 2004 ; Nori et al, 2006 ; Nori and Piccardi, 2015 ). This is evident in tasks where they have to indicate how to reach a target: men normally provide cardinal points and distance information whereas women prefer to add information about landmarks (Miller and Santoni, 1986 ; Ward et al, 1986 ; Schmitz, 1997 ; Dabbs et al, 1998 ; Denis et al, 1999 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These strategies are named “spatial styles.” Some studies have found that women are usually prone to use “landmark” or “route styles,” where egocentric information is necessary. On the contrary, men usually choose a “survey style,” which implies allocentric information and capacity to represent the environment as a map (Coluccia and Louse, 2004 ; Nori et al, 2006 ; Nori and Piccardi, 2015 ). This is evident in tasks where they have to indicate how to reach a target: men normally provide cardinal points and distance information whereas women prefer to add information about landmarks (Miller and Santoni, 1986 ; Ward et al, 1986 ; Schmitz, 1997 ; Dabbs et al, 1998 ; Denis et al, 1999 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although gender differences are not uniformly found, when they are found they often favour males [3,49,53]. However, gender difference is still preserves its' uncertainty to this day because there are many individual differences and other factors that may have effect on wayfinding and spatial navigation [10,[13][14][15][16][17]35,48,58,62,63,67,79,82].…”
Section: Gender Difference and Wayfinding Performancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Wayfinding anxiety has been associated with poorer performance, lower self-efficacy, and less pleasure in exploring in spatial tasks (Lawton, 1994; Coluccia and Louse, 2004; Pazzaglia et al, 2018). Interestingly, even though women have been described as being more anxious than men when wayfinding (Lawton, 1994, 1996; Schmitz, 1997; Schug, 2016) and having less self-confidence to solve spatial tasks (Picucci et al, 2011; Nori and Piccardi, 2015), they perform comparably and achieve similar results as men in spatial tasks such as mental rotation tasks (Neuburger et al, 2012), wayfinding tasks (Lawton, 1996), and spatial environmental tasks (Nori and Piccardi, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%