Warmup effects, the repeated within-session transitions from ineffective to effective avoidance, were examined with rats on free-operant shock-delay procedures. The shock-shock and response-shock intervals were kept equal as they were varied. As measured by both response rates and shock rates, the magnitude of within-session change in performance was inversely related to the size of the manipulated intervals. The duration of warmup tended to decrease as the intervals were increased. This finding, that increased shock frequencies do not shorten the warmup, appears to be inconsistent with all interpretations of the warmup that have been offered to date. Late-session performances replicated general features of prior experiments, but differed with respect to details of secondary conclusions in previous reports. These differences may stem from the selection of especially proficient avoiders for previous experiments.Key words: warmup in avoidance, free-operant avoidance, shock frequency, subject selection, lever press, rats Soon after devising his well-known freeoperant avoidance procedure based on shock delay (Sidman, 1953a), Sidman systematically examined the two main parameters of that procedure (Sidman, 1953b). Those are the response-shock interval, which is the amount of delay producible by a single response, and the shock-shock interval, which is the time between shocks if no responses intervene. He recorded the rates of responding while varying the response-shock interval, in conjunction with each of several shock-shock intervals. Subsequently, other investigators covered part of the same ground, varying the response-shock (RS) and shock-shock (SS) intervals while keeping them equal to each other (SS = RS). Using a single rat, Verhave (1959) (1966) examined intervals ranging from 10 to 60 sec. All four experiments produced functions of approximately the same shape, resembling hyperbolic, or logarithmic relations between response rate and the SS = RS interval; their data gave approximately linear functions when rate was plotted against the reciprocal of the interval, at least over the ranges of intervals that were used.While these results are reliable and useful for many purposes, they provide a very circumscribed picture of the behavior produced by free-operant avoidance procedures. Of the four studies, only the one covering the narrowest range of SS = RS intervals (Herrnstein and Brady, 1958) analyzed data from whole sessions. The other three studies omitted from analysis one-third, one-half, or two-thirds of the sessions of asymptotic performance that were chosen for data analysis. Further, Clark and Hull's experiment used preselected animals that had shown especially proficient performance. To be sure, this kind of selectivity in avoidance experiments has not been limited to studies of SS and RS intervals. Whatever avoidance variables are under examination, it is common practice to delete animals that do not avoid proficiently, and to discard the early-