2007
DOI: 10.3758/bf03193953
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Ignorance is bliss: The role of observer expectation in dynamic spatial tuning of the attentional focus

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
57
4

Year Published

2007
2007
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

5
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
5
57
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, the present study confirms and extends earlier work by Jefferies et al (2007), who found a complete AB, including considerable lag 1 sparing, across multiple locations when participants had no preknowledge on the location of T1. Jefferies et al argued that the uncertainty of T1's location led observers to adopt a wider attentional focus, so that T2 could often pass through (at lag 1) regardless of its location.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Finally, the present study confirms and extends earlier work by Jefferies et al (2007), who found a complete AB, including considerable lag 1 sparing, across multiple locations when participants had no preknowledge on the location of T1. Jefferies et al argued that the uncertainty of T1's location led observers to adopt a wider attentional focus, so that T2 could often pass through (at lag 1) regardless of its location.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…We have no clear explanation for the lack of recovery here, other than that the lags were insufficiently long to allow for recovery or observers occasionally give up looking at the steam altogether when T2 has not arrived within a certain time. We point out that a lack of recovery is not unique (e.g., Jefferies et al, 2007;Nieuwenhuis, Gilzenrat, Holmes, & Cohen, 2005;Shih, 2000).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Duncan et al (1994) presented two masked targets at different spatiotemporal offsets and observed what they termed an attentional dwell time, which resembles in many ways an attentional blink, except that the targets were presented approximately 2-4 degrees apart and without RSVP. Many other experiments have demonstrated variations of the attentional blink across different locations for targets presented either in isolation, or in ongoing RSVP streams with spatial separations ranging from .5 to 9 degrees of visual angle (Bay & Wyble, 2014;Du, Abrams, & Zhang, 2011;Jefferies & Di Lollo, 2009;Jefferies, Enns, & Di Lollo, 2014;Kawahara & Yamada, 2006;Jefferies, Ghorashi, Kawahara, & Di Lollo, 2007;Kristjánsson & Nakayama, 2002;Shih, 2000;Visser, Bischof, & Di Lollo, 1999).…”
Section: The Attentional Blink and The Attentional Dwell Timementioning
confidence: 97%
“…It has been shown that Lag-1 sparing also occurs to targets in different spatial locations, but only if T2 falls within the focus of attention (Jefferies & Di Lollo, 2009;Jefferies, Ghorashi, Kawahara, & Di Lollo, 2007;Shih, 2000). The incidence of Lag-1 sparing can, therefore, be used to determine whether or not a particular spatial location falls within the focus of attention and thus to assess the distribution of attention across space (Jefferies & Di Lollo, 2009;Jefferies, Ghorashi, Kawahara, & Di Lollo, 2007; Kawahara & Yamada, 2007;Lunau & Olivers, 2010).Our implementation of the AB and Lag-1 sparing was based on the methodology used by Kawahara and Yamada (2007), who employed two concurrent streams of digit distractors, one on either side of fixation, separated by a spatial gap. A first pair of letter-targets (T1-pair) appeared simultaneously, one in each stream (in-stream condition).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%