1998
DOI: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2442
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Implicit Sequence Learning in Children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

35
211
12
20

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 207 publications
(278 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
35
211
12
20
Order By: Relevance
“…But the basic idea underlying this study is that, even if it is proved that explicit mechanisms may play a role in SRT performance, this does not mean that the SRT paradigm is an explicit task per se. Our hypothesis is that participants' ability to use explicit strategies in the SRT task depends on the characteristics of the task, including the RSI, the kind of instructions given to participants, or the disruption of the sequence by the interpolation of random elements between each presentation of the deterministic sequence (e.g., Meulemans et al, 1998). the PI explicit learning group processed the regular trials quicker.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…But the basic idea underlying this study is that, even if it is proved that explicit mechanisms may play a role in SRT performance, this does not mean that the SRT paradigm is an explicit task per se. Our hypothesis is that participants' ability to use explicit strategies in the SRT task depends on the characteristics of the task, including the RSI, the kind of instructions given to participants, or the disruption of the sequence by the interpolation of random elements between each presentation of the deterministic sequence (e.g., Meulemans et al, 1998). the PI explicit learning group processed the regular trials quicker.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other evidence comes from studies showing a dissociation between performance on the SRT task and performance on a subsequent explicit task (e.g., a recognition or generation task). If participants show sequence learning in the SRT task and no sequence knowledge in an explicit task, this result can be viewed as an argument in favor of the existence of sequence-learning mechanisms that occur outside of consciousness (e.g., Meulemans, Van der Linden, & Perruchet, 1998;Perruchet, Bigand, & BenoitGonin, 1997). However, these data must always be treated with caution.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, the literature is more divided. Some results suggest that implicit learning skills are developmentally invariant; that is, that they are detectable in early infancy (Fiser & Aslin, 2002;Saffran, 2003;Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996), and that they do not differ or differ only slightly between children and adults (e.g., Cherry & Stadler, 1995;Howard & Howard, 1989;Meulemans, Van der Linden, & Perruchet, 1998;Seger, 1994;Thomas & Nelson, 2001). Other results find evidence that implicit learning skills do change over childhood (Janacsek, Fiser, & Nemeth, 2012;Thomas et al, 2004), particularly for higher-order patterns (Howard & Howard, 1997).…”
Section: Theoretical Concerns About the Auditory Scaffolding Hypothmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Of these, the most salient is the Serial Reaction Time (SRT) task (Nissen & Bullemer, 1987), which can also be used with children (Meulemans et al, 1998). It has been found to be robustly sensitive to individual and group differences in language processing (Kidd, 2012;Tomblin et al, 2007) as well as higher-order cognitive skills (Kaufman et al, 2010).…”
Section: Empirical Concerns About the Auditory Scaffolding Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After some amount of practice, performance of participants on the repeated sequence is generally better than performance on a new sequence. In recent years, a huge number of studies using SRT tasks have improved our knowledge about various issues, such as the implicit-learning abilities of children (e.g., Meulemans, Van Der Linden, & Perruchet, 1998), elderly people (e.g., Curran, 1997), and neurologically impaired patients (e.g., Smith, Siegert, McDowall, & Abernethy, 2001), the relationships between performance and explicit knowledge (e.g., Shanks & Perruchet, 2002), and the relationships between perceptual and motor components of learning (e.g., Kelly, Burton, Riedel, & Lynch, 2003).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%