2002
DOI: 10.1097/00006324-200201000-00013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Improving the Effectiveness of the Infant Contrast Sensitivity Card Procedure

Abstract: Based on results from an earlier prototype, custom software and printing techniques were developed to construct a new card-based test of contrast sensitivity (CS) for nonverbal subjects. Compared with the prototype, the new CS card test contains three improvements: (1) larger, more salient test gratings; (2) higher contrast warm-up cards for each spatial frequency set; and (3) smaller contrast step size between adjacent cards. The success of the new cards was evaluated by testing 3.5- and 12-month-old human in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Others have argued that the immature foveal cones directly cause the high spatial frequency loss28, but we think it more likely that the immature fovea forces infants to use their more mature extrafoveal vision for most visual tasks29. This issue can be examined by comparing the free-fixating infant CSF30 to the CSF of extrafoveally-tested adults31, 32. When the adult CSF is measured at approximately the eccentricity of the grating stimuli relative to the center of the preferential looking apparatus (about 7.5 deg), infant and adult CSFs are similar in shape, and can be more-or-less well superimposed by a vertical shift (relative to a logarithmic Y-axis) of about 0.75 log units (Fig.…”
Section: Infant Photopic Sensitivity To Contrastmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Others have argued that the immature foveal cones directly cause the high spatial frequency loss28, but we think it more likely that the immature fovea forces infants to use their more mature extrafoveal vision for most visual tasks29. This issue can be examined by comparing the free-fixating infant CSF30 to the CSF of extrafoveally-tested adults31, 32. When the adult CSF is measured at approximately the eccentricity of the grating stimuli relative to the center of the preferential looking apparatus (about 7.5 deg), infant and adult CSFs are similar in shape, and can be more-or-less well superimposed by a vertical shift (relative to a logarithmic Y-axis) of about 0.75 log units (Fig.…”
Section: Infant Photopic Sensitivity To Contrastmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Black diamonds, monocular data30, white diamonds, binocular data30. Black triangles, adult temporal monocular data (eccentricity=7.5 deg)31, white triangles, adult nasal data (eccentricity=10 deg)31.…”
Section: Figuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Behavioral tests developed to assess CS in infants and children are more straightforward to administer in the clinic because the examiner directly assesses the child's "looking" response. Pediatric CS test stimuli include sine-wave gratings (contrast sensitivity card test [CSCT]), [23][24][25][26] a large low frequency square wave grating (OCC), 9 schematic face stimuli (Hiding Heidi 27,28 [HH], www.good-lite.com; Mr. Happy 29 ), and picture outlines (Cardiff Contrast Test 30,31 [CCT], www.eyesfirst.eu).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…17–22 Test results were classified as normal if they fell within 95% tolerance limits for age, borderline if they fell outside the 95% tolerance limit but within the 99% tolerance limit, and abnormal if they fell outside of the 99% tolerance limit 18,21,22 …”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For 12-month-olds and for 4-year-olds 95% tolerance ranges for normal contrast sensitivity 18,21 are shown as the pale gray areas.…”
Section: Figmentioning
confidence: 99%