2014
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-014-0412-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Incidental encoding of enclosure geometry does not require visual input: evidence from blindfolded adults

Abstract: Although spatial orientation with respect to the geometric properties of an environment appears to be an ability shared across various species, debate remains concerning potential similarities and differences with respect to the underlying mechanism(s). One prominent theoretical account of orientation with respect to the environment suggests that participants match visual memories to their current visual perception and navigate to reduce the discrepancy between the two. We tested whether visual input was neces… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, with visual impairments or under conditions of low environment visibility (e.g., under foggy conditions or at night), anecdotal experience suggests that reorientation can be accomplished using auditory (e.g., traffic noise), balance/kinesthetic (e.g., walking on a slant), or haptic (e.g., long cane) information. Recently, these experiences have been supported through systematic studies (reorientation using auditory cues: Nardi, Anzures, et al, 2019; kinesthetic cues from slope information: Nardi et al, 2011; and haptics: Sturz et al, 2014), but what we know about how these other sensory modalities support reorientation, compared to visually guided reorientation, is very limited. This knowledge is useful because it can be employed to design multimodal spatial displays to help everyday navigation and safe mobility (e.g., maps, crossing signals, exit localisation).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, with visual impairments or under conditions of low environment visibility (e.g., under foggy conditions or at night), anecdotal experience suggests that reorientation can be accomplished using auditory (e.g., traffic noise), balance/kinesthetic (e.g., walking on a slant), or haptic (e.g., long cane) information. Recently, these experiences have been supported through systematic studies (reorientation using auditory cues: Nardi, Anzures, et al, 2019; kinesthetic cues from slope information: Nardi et al, 2011; and haptics: Sturz et al, 2014), but what we know about how these other sensory modalities support reorientation, compared to visually guided reorientation, is very limited. This knowledge is useful because it can be employed to design multimodal spatial displays to help everyday navigation and safe mobility (e.g., maps, crossing signals, exit localisation).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a rectangular environment with a distinct textural cue in each corner, participants learned to associate the correct cue to the target corner. Furthermore, and crucially, even if not necessary for the task, they also extracted the geometric information from the environment so that, when the texture cues were removed, they were able to restrict searches to the two, opposite, geometrically correct corners (Sturz et al, 2014). We were interested in examining whether the incidental encoding of geometry would also occur with an auditory accessed environment.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our expectation was based on previous studies on visual-based reorientation, which overall reveal that a source of spatial information—especially a geometric cue—tends to be encoded even when not necessary or sufficient (e.g., Cheng, 1986; Cheng et al, 2013). This has been shown even with haptic cues (Sturz et al, 2014). In contrast, in Experiment 1, there was no evidence that the auditory information was encoded at all.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…In a similar scenario but with visual cues, abundant evidence (Hermer & Spelke, 1996; Learmonth, Newcombe, & Huttenlocher, 2001; Vallortigara, Zanforlin, & Pasti, 1990; for a review, see Cheng & Newcombe, 2005) has shown that human and non-human animals tend to learn the target location with both sources of information and not with just the more informative one. Sturz and colleagues (2014) revealed that haptic-based reorientation followed the same pattern: participants learned both the target texture cue and the geometrically correct location, although the texture information alone would have sufficed.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation