2020
DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00291
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Increased Goal Tracking in Adolescent Rats Is Goal-Directed and Not Habit-Like

Abstract: When a cue is paired with reward in a different location, some animals will approach the site of reward during the cue, a behavior called goal tracking, while other animals will approach and interact with the cue itself: a behavior called sign tracking. Sign tracking is thought to reflect a tendency to transfer incentive salience from the reward to the cue. Adolescence is a time of heightened sensitivity to rewards, including environmental cues that have been associated with rewards, which may account for incr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

3
12
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
(74 reference statements)
3
12
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The current findings may also relate to potential differences between adolescent and adult rats in the behavioral systems they relied on to control their reward-seeking behavior. For example, recent devaluation studies suggest that adolescent rats are resistant to developing habits, both in terms of their instrumental lever pressing ( Serlin and Torregrossa, 2015 ; Towner et al, 2020 ) and Pavlovian conditioned approach behavior ( Rode et al, 2020 ). However, this does not readily account for the current findings given that reward-paired cues are more effective in motivating instrumental performance under conditions that promote habitual control ( Holland, 2004 ; Wiltgen et al, 2012 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The current findings may also relate to potential differences between adolescent and adult rats in the behavioral systems they relied on to control their reward-seeking behavior. For example, recent devaluation studies suggest that adolescent rats are resistant to developing habits, both in terms of their instrumental lever pressing ( Serlin and Torregrossa, 2015 ; Towner et al, 2020 ) and Pavlovian conditioned approach behavior ( Rode et al, 2020 ). However, this does not readily account for the current findings given that reward-paired cues are more effective in motivating instrumental performance under conditions that promote habitual control ( Holland, 2004 ; Wiltgen et al, 2012 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Taken together, the BLA and NAc support incentive learning relying on both conditioned stimulus (CS) value and current outcome (US) value. A growing number of studies demonstrate that GT, but not ST, rats flexibly reduce approach after outcome devaluation induced by satiety or illness (Morrison et al, 2015 ; Nasser et al, 2015 ; Patitucci et al, 2016 ; Smedley and Smith, 2018 ; Rode et al, 2020 ; Keefer et al, 2020 ). Both ST and GT rats similarly acquire and express SOC (Nasser et al, 2015 ; Saddoris et al, 2016 ), learning that requires BLA–NAc communication.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This unexpected finding could suggest that patch activation directly drives habitual reward seeking. Previous studies have utilized head entry as a metric for habitual responding and reward devaluation has been shown to reduce head entry in goal-directed animals (DePoy et al, 2016; Morrison et al, 2015; Rode et al, 2020). Discrete head entry and lever pressing events could be ‘chunked’ into larger learned action sequences that are reinforced across habit formation (Dezfouli and Balleine, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%