2015
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4328-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Independent effects of reward expectation and spatial orientation on the processing of emotional facial expressions

Abstract: The present study investigated the effect of reward expectation and spatial orientation on the processing of emotional facial expressions, using a spatial cue-target paradigm. A colored cue was presented at the left or right side of the central fixation point, with its color indicating the monetary reward stakes of a given trial (incentive vs. non-incentive), followed by the presentation of an emotional facial target (angry vs. neutral) at a cued or un-cued location. Participants were asked to discriminate the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

2
4
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
2
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A cue indicating reward delivery for successful performance would activate the motivational component of reward, which could elicit behavior changes (Notebaert & Braem, 2015). A number of studies suggest that reward-induced motivation promotes behavior performance and enhances cognitive control (Botvinick & Braver, 2015;Chiew & Braver, 2016;Kang, Zhou, & Wei, 2015;Padmala & Pessoa, 2011;Pessoa, 2009;Soutschek et al, 2015;Wei & Kang, 2014). Extending the evidence in the visual domain, our findings demonstrate that reward can enhance conflict resolution in the cross-modal context, reducing cross-modal conflict regardless of the target modality.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 75%
“…A cue indicating reward delivery for successful performance would activate the motivational component of reward, which could elicit behavior changes (Notebaert & Braem, 2015). A number of studies suggest that reward-induced motivation promotes behavior performance and enhances cognitive control (Botvinick & Braver, 2015;Chiew & Braver, 2016;Kang, Zhou, & Wei, 2015;Padmala & Pessoa, 2011;Pessoa, 2009;Soutschek et al, 2015;Wei & Kang, 2014). Extending the evidence in the visual domain, our findings demonstrate that reward can enhance conflict resolution in the cross-modal context, reducing cross-modal conflict regardless of the target modality.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 75%
“…This indicates that reward-induced motivation had a clear effect on inhibitory processes following the initial capture of attention. Rutherford et al (2010) found that there was a significant difference in IOR between rewarded and unrewarded conditions with a long SOA, and IOR was significantly enhanced (Kang et al, 2015;Rutherford et al, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Reward learning modifies the attentional priority of stimuli involuntarily, enabling them to compete more effectively for selection (Anderson, 2013;Anderson et al, 2011;Failing & Theeuwes, 2014Hickey et al, 2010Hickey et al, , 2011. Studies have used the spatial cue-target paradigm to investigate the impact of reward on attention and found that reward affects the inhibition of spatial attention: the reaction times (RTs) of low-reward condition are typically longer than those of high-reward condition (Blini et al, 2018;Bucker & Theeuwes, 2014;Kang et al, 2015;Munneke et al, 2015;Rutherford et al, 2010). Munneke et al (2015) found that high reward stimuli captured more attention and resulted in sped-up RTs when rewarded cues signaled the correct target location with cue-target task in which both endogenous and exogenous cues were used.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As background to the aforementioned effects of reward expectation and emotional processing on attention and executive control, recently several studies have attempted to directly examine the relationship between reward expectation and explicit and/or implicit emotional processing (Kaltwasser, Ries, Sommer, Knight, & Willems, ; Padmala & Pessoa, ; Padmala, Sirbu, & Pessoa, ; Wei & Kang, ; Wei, Wang, & Ji, ). When participants were instructed to discriminate the emotional valence of emotional stimuli (i.e., explicit emotional processing) following incentive or nonincentive cues, behavioral and electrophysiological evidence revealed that reward expectation in the incentive (vs. the nonincentive) condition amplifies emotional effects—that is, the preferential processing of negative and positive emotional stimuli versus neutral stimuli (Kang, Zhou, & Wei, ; Wei & Kang, ; Wei et al, ; Wei, Kang, Ding, & Guo, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%