2013
DOI: 10.5539/jedp.v3n1p192
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Individual Differences in Children’s Early Strategy Behavior in Arithmetic Tasks

Abstract: As demonstrated by the Overlapping Waves Model (Siegler, 1996), children's strategy use in arithmetic tasks is variable, adaptive, and changes gradually with age and experience. In this study, first grade elementary school children (n = 73), who scored high, middle, or low in a standardized scholastic mathematic achievement test, were confronted with different arithmetic tasks (simple addition, e.g., 3 + 2, simple subtraction, e.g., 7 -2, and more advanced addition, e.g., 7 + 9) to evoke different calculation … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Children can arrive at accurate and inaccurate solutions to addition problems through a variety of strategies. Examining children’s strategies provides greater insight into their understanding of arithmetic principles and numerical magnitudes than examining only accuracy (Canobi, Reeve, & Pattison, 2003; Carpenter, Franke, Jacobs, Fennema, & Empson, 1998; Geary, Bow-Thomas, & Yao, 1992; Lindberg, Linkersdörfer, Lehmann, Hasselhorn, & Lonnemann, 2013). In fact, how children solve problems has been found to be more predictive of later mathematics achievement than the accuracy with which they solve problems (Geary, 2011).…”
Section: Addition Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children can arrive at accurate and inaccurate solutions to addition problems through a variety of strategies. Examining children’s strategies provides greater insight into their understanding of arithmetic principles and numerical magnitudes than examining only accuracy (Canobi, Reeve, & Pattison, 2003; Carpenter, Franke, Jacobs, Fennema, & Empson, 1998; Geary, Bow-Thomas, & Yao, 1992; Lindberg, Linkersdörfer, Lehmann, Hasselhorn, & Lonnemann, 2013). In fact, how children solve problems has been found to be more predictive of later mathematics achievement than the accuracy with which they solve problems (Geary, 2011).…”
Section: Addition Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, the growth in arithmetic fluency for children in grade 2 may be reflected in better execution of both min and decomposition strategies. Notably, in the present research, I did not collect children's strategy data for the addition tasks, thus, the assumption that the improvement of the arithmetic fluency for children in grades 1 and 2 resulted from the increasing use and execution of more efficient counting and decomposition strategies was not directly tested because there is already a wealth of evidence showing how children's strategies develop as they become skilled at simple addition (e.g., Geary et al, 2004;Lindberg et al, 2013;Siegler, 1987;Shrager & Siegler, 1998;Siegler & Araya, 2005).…”
Section: Hypothesis 1: Integration Status Of Ordinal Associationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On this view, ordinal processing can be viewed as the higher-level processing in the associative network than cardinal processing, because it involves a wider range of cognitive skills (cardinal, sequential and inhibitory processing). Further, arithmetic associations are integrated into the hierarchical network as arithmetic solutions require children to access their sequential and cardinal associations and thus arithmetic fluency is built upon fluent access to ordinal associations (Geary et al, 2004;Lindberg et al, 2013;Shrager & Siegler, 1998). Thus, the integration of cardinal and sequential associations into ordinal associations is critical for the development of mental arithmetic skills for children.…”
Section: Performance By Grade Andmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations