2014
DOI: 10.1111/infa.12049
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Individual Differences in Infant Oculomotor Behavior During the Viewing of Complex Naturalistic Scenes

Abstract: Little research hitherto has examined how individual differences in attention, as assessed using standard experimental paradigms, relate to individual differences in how attention is spontaneously allocated in more naturalistic contexts. Here, we analyzed the time intervals between refoveating eye movements (fixation durations) while typically developing 11-month-old infants viewed a 90-min battery ranging from complex dynamic to noncomplex static materials. The same infants also completed experimental assessm… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
86
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 72 publications
(95 citation statements)
references
References 114 publications
(237 reference statements)
8
86
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One telling comparison may be to liken the present findings, that compare infant attention during Joint Play and Solo Play, with previous findings that compare infant attention towards static and dynamic screen stimuli (Courage et al, 2006;Richards, 2010;Shaddy & Colombo, 2004). At the same time, the exogenous influences on gaze behaviour during the viewing of dynamic stimuli are thought to be stronger (Courage et al, 2006;Wass & Smith, 2014a, 2014b, and, most likely because of this, infants' attention durations towards dynamic stimuli are markedly higher than towards static stimuli (Shaddy & Colombo, 2004 (Carpenter et al, 1998), along with superior visual attention control (Niedźwiecka et al, 2017)? At the same time, the exogenous influences on gaze behaviour during the viewing of dynamic stimuli are thought to be stronger (Courage et al, 2006;Wass & Smith, 2014a, 2014b, and, most likely because of this, infants' attention durations towards dynamic stimuli are markedly higher than towards static stimuli (Shaddy & Colombo, 2004 (Carpenter et al, 1998), along with superior visual attention control (Niedźwiecka et al, 2017)?…”
Section: Procedures 2 Which Directly Compared the Transitions Betweensupporting
confidence: 64%
“…One telling comparison may be to liken the present findings, that compare infant attention during Joint Play and Solo Play, with previous findings that compare infant attention towards static and dynamic screen stimuli (Courage et al, 2006;Richards, 2010;Shaddy & Colombo, 2004). At the same time, the exogenous influences on gaze behaviour during the viewing of dynamic stimuli are thought to be stronger (Courage et al, 2006;Wass & Smith, 2014a, 2014b, and, most likely because of this, infants' attention durations towards dynamic stimuli are markedly higher than towards static stimuli (Shaddy & Colombo, 2004 (Carpenter et al, 1998), along with superior visual attention control (Niedźwiecka et al, 2017)? At the same time, the exogenous influences on gaze behaviour during the viewing of dynamic stimuli are thought to be stronger (Courage et al, 2006;Wass & Smith, 2014a, 2014b, and, most likely because of this, infants' attention durations towards dynamic stimuli are markedly higher than towards static stimuli (Shaddy & Colombo, 2004 (Carpenter et al, 1998), along with superior visual attention control (Niedźwiecka et al, 2017)?…”
Section: Procedures 2 Which Directly Compared the Transitions Betweensupporting
confidence: 64%
“…We know that infants show longer looks while observing complex or dynamic images as well as films, and that they show shorter looks when observing static or simpler images (Courage et al, 2006;Wass & Smith, 2014). It is possible that, during a session, looking measures are independently affected by differences between the tasks as well as by changing arousal levels.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Researchers have yet to compare toddlers’ eye movements when watching the same events on video versus in person, particularly as related to subsequent learning. Moreover, while toddlers (15–30 months) are likely to exhibit a video deficit for learning (Barr, ), most eye‐tracking studies that use video stimuli are limited to younger infants and adults (e.g., Frank et al., ; Kirkorian et al., ; Mital, Smith, Hill, & Henderson, ; Taylor & Herbert, , ; Wass & Smith, ). Thus, it remains unclear whether the video deficit is at least partly the result of differences in online processing during knowledge acquisition when viewing video versus in‐person events.…”
Section: Online Processing Of Video Versus In‐person Eventsmentioning
confidence: 99%