2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-016-0687-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Individual differences in spontaneous analogical transfer

Abstract: Research on analogical problem solving has shown that people often fail to spontaneously notice the relevance of a semantically remote source analog when solving a target problem, although they are able to form mappings and derive inferences when given a hint to recall the source. Relatively little work has investigated possible individual differences that predict spontaneous transfer, or how such differences may interact with interventions that facilitate transfer. In this study, fluid intelligence was measur… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
24
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
4
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, imposing a workingmemory load causes college students to make fewer relational (and more featural) matches on a picture-mapping task (Waltz, Lau, Grewal, & Holyoak, 2000). Scores on the Ravens Progressive Matrices (RPM), a standard measure of fluid intelligence (Raven, 1938), have been shown to correlate positively with the probability of spontaneous analogical transfer in a problem-solving task (Kubricht, Lu, & Holyoak, 2017). Neuropsychological evidence links impaired prefrontal functioning with greatly diminished performance on analogy tasks (e.g., Kane & Engle, 2002;Krawczyk et al, 2008;Morrison et al, 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, imposing a workingmemory load causes college students to make fewer relational (and more featural) matches on a picture-mapping task (Waltz, Lau, Grewal, & Holyoak, 2000). Scores on the Ravens Progressive Matrices (RPM), a standard measure of fluid intelligence (Raven, 1938), have been shown to correlate positively with the probability of spontaneous analogical transfer in a problem-solving task (Kubricht, Lu, & Holyoak, 2017). Neuropsychological evidence links impaired prefrontal functioning with greatly diminished performance on analogy tasks (e.g., Kane & Engle, 2002;Krawczyk et al, 2008;Morrison et al, 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather, success on the RAT was the only significant predictor of completeness. This result was somewhat unexpected as WMC, and the closely related construct of fluid intelligence, have been found to relate to advantages in the construction of text and problem representation (Andersson, 2007;Daneman & Carpenter, 1980;Friedman & Miyake, 2000;Kubricht, Lu, & Holyoak, 2016;Singer & Ritchot, 1996;Swanson, Cooney, & Brock, 1993;Thevenot & Oakhill, 2006;Turner & Engle, 1989).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In particular, while previous research has used load-based (e.g., Waltz, Lau, Grewal, & Holyoak, 2000), or non-equivalent group designs (e.g., Richland et al, 2006Richland et al, , 2010Viskontas et al, 2004), this study used a regressionbased approach to explore how individual differences in attentional control and the ability to make remote associations might relate to successful analogical transfer, and to test the hypothesis that both abilities should predict spontaneous solutions in an analogical transfer paradigm. This approach is aligned with other recent work by Kubricht, Lu, and Holyoak (2016), but uses different individual difference measures. The use of multiple measures in this area of research is important to establish convergent validity as to what constructs are relevant.…”
Section: Overview Of the Present Studymentioning
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations