2017
DOI: 10.1037/emo0000307
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infant differential behavioral responding to discrete emotions.

Abstract: Emotional communication regulates the behaviors of social partners. Research on individuals' responding to others' emotions typically compares responses to a single negative emotion compared with responses to a neutral or positive emotion. Furthermore, coding of such responses routinely measure surface level features of the behavior (e.g., approach vs. avoidance) rather than its underlying function (e.g., the goal of the approach or avoidant behavior). This investigation examined infants' responding to others'… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
61
1
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
(101 reference statements)
1
61
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Conversely, parents talked about the referent more in disgust, fear, and joy contexts than in sadness and anger contexts. These differences in parent talk about discrete emotions mirror some findings of infant behavioral responses to such contexts, specifically infants' physical avoidance of disgust referents, engagement with sad emoters, and social avoidance of angry emoters (Walle, Reschke, Camras, et al, ). However, it should be emphasized that although the frequency of parents' mentioning the emoter and the referent varied across discrete emotions, this does not signify that parents talked exclusively about one element or the other.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Conversely, parents talked about the referent more in disgust, fear, and joy contexts than in sadness and anger contexts. These differences in parent talk about discrete emotions mirror some findings of infant behavioral responses to such contexts, specifically infants' physical avoidance of disgust referents, engagement with sad emoters, and social avoidance of angry emoters (Walle, Reschke, Camras, et al, ). However, it should be emphasized that although the frequency of parents' mentioning the emoter and the referent varied across discrete emotions, this does not signify that parents talked exclusively about one element or the other.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…For example, increased parent focus on the emoter in sadness contexts may influence infant's prosocial responding to sad individuals whereas focus on a disgusting referent may result in physical avoidance of the object. Further study of how parent talk about discrete emotions corresponds with differential behavioral responding to such emotions (Walle, Reschke, Camras, et al, ) can inform our understanding of how parent socialization of emotion corresponds with infant functional affective responding (see Walle & Campos, ). More broadly, considering qualitative differences in the value and function of discrete emotions will sharpen our focus on how socialization practices, such as parent talk about emotions, facilitate emotional development.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Thus, we explored whether infants' prosocial behavior varied (a) in response to a specific discrete emotion across age groups, and (b) across discrete emotions within a particular age group. For example, infants' use of indirect helping in response to an angry adult might be most prevalent by 24-month-old infants, an age group previously found to demonstrate more differentiated behavioral responses to this emotion than younger infants (see Walle et al, 2017). However, we refrained from making specific hypotheses for each possible comparison given the exploratory nature of the analyses.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%