2001
DOI: 10.1002/dev.1011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infants' long‐term memory for a serial list: Recognition and reactivation

Abstract: Serial lists contain information about item identity and item order. Using a task designed for nonverbal animals, we previously found that 3- and 6-month-olds exhibited a primacy effect after 24 hr, remembering both item identity and item order. Presently, we examined their memory of list information after longer delays. In Experiment 1, the serial-position curve reverted to a U-shape after 1 week at both ages, revealing that the common practice of attributing primacy and recency effects to long- and short-ter… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, one study presented 7‐month‐old infants with a sequence of three faces and found that after a short delay, infants recognized the faces presented first and last, but not the face presented in the middle of the sequence. Other studies have demonstrated long‐term memory serial position effects in 3‐ and 6‐month‐olds using small visual patterns (Gulya, Sweeney & Rovee‐Collier, ; Gulya, Galluccio, Wilk & Rovee‐Collier, ; Gulya, Rovee‐Collier, Galluccio & Wilk, ). Some research also provides evidence of an enhanced encoding of edges of linguistic stimuli.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…For example, one study presented 7‐month‐old infants with a sequence of three faces and found that after a short delay, infants recognized the faces presented first and last, but not the face presented in the middle of the sequence. Other studies have demonstrated long‐term memory serial position effects in 3‐ and 6‐month‐olds using small visual patterns (Gulya, Sweeney & Rovee‐Collier, ; Gulya, Galluccio, Wilk & Rovee‐Collier, ; Gulya, Rovee‐Collier, Galluccio & Wilk, ). Some research also provides evidence of an enhanced encoding of edges of linguistic stimuli.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…We have found that a reminder reactivates not only the original memory but also memories that are associated with it by virtue of sharing a common context or cue (Gulya, Galluccio, Wilk, & Rovee‐Collier, ; Shields & Rovee‐Collier, ; Timmons, ). Although acquisition and extinction had occurred in two different physical contexts in Experiment 2a, these two manipulations had also occurred in the same temporal context (Bouton, ).…”
Section: Experiments 2b: Temporally Discrete Extinctionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rose et al (2001) tested infants’ memory for all the items in the list; infants’ memory for the third item, therefore, was tested after their memory for the first and second item had been tested. Gulya, Sweeney, and Rovee-Collier (2001) used a very different procedure and found that when infants were trained to kick their leg to make different mobiles move (thus the “lists” were a series of mobiles presented in sequence on three daily training sessions), 3- and 6-month-old infants’ memory for the mobiles showed position effects after a 1-week delay. The point is that, like adults, infants’ memory for particular items in a study list is influenced by item position in the list.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%