2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.05.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Infants’ use of category knowledge and object attributes when segregating objects at 8.5 months of age

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We think it unlikely that labels are required for the formation of object categories, however, since even young infants form and use object categories (e.g., Eimas & Quinn, 1994; Needham, Cantlon, & Ormsbee Holley, 2006; Needham, Dueker, & Lockhead, 2005; Quinn, Eimas, & Rosenkrantz, 1993). Instead, as discussed later, our claims are that (1) although young infants who encounter an artifact in a nonfunctional context typically do not encode its object category, they can readily be primed to do so do by various non-linguistic manipulations, and (2) toward the end of the first year, perhaps as a result of language acquisition, infants begin to spontaneously encode artifacts taxonomically, even in non-functional contexts and without priming manipulations.…”
Section: Object Individuation In Infantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We think it unlikely that labels are required for the formation of object categories, however, since even young infants form and use object categories (e.g., Eimas & Quinn, 1994; Needham, Cantlon, & Ormsbee Holley, 2006; Needham, Dueker, & Lockhead, 2005; Quinn, Eimas, & Rosenkrantz, 1993). Instead, as discussed later, our claims are that (1) although young infants who encounter an artifact in a nonfunctional context typically do not encode its object category, they can readily be primed to do so do by various non-linguistic manipulations, and (2) toward the end of the first year, perhaps as a result of language acquisition, infants begin to spontaneously encode artifacts taxonomically, even in non-functional contexts and without priming manipulations.…”
Section: Object Individuation In Infantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Each object representation is bound to its index [16] so that infants can keep track of the properties of each object. A variety of segregation, recognition and categorization processes can operate on the representations, to include additional information or to highlight particular information [23][24][25][26][27]. For example, infants might use information from previous encounters with the cover (or similar covers) to determine that it is a cover rather than a block.…”
Section: A Three-system Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, experience and experience‐dependent preferences seem to play a role in categorization performance (Needham et al., 2006, discuss related findings), and preexisting representations may help to explain categorization in young infants. More generally, these findings suggest that infants’ experiences outside the laboratory can affect their categorical responding in the laboratory (Bar‐Haim, Ziv, Lamy, & Hodes, 2006; Kelly et al., 2005, 2007).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%