The Oxford Handbook of Information Structure 2014
DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199642670.013.011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Information Structure in First Language Acquisition

Abstract: So far, research on the acquisition of information structure (IS) is still relatively sparse compared to other areas of first language acquisition research. A growing interest in this area has emerged with an increasing number of results indicating an asymmetry of an early production but late comprehension of linguistic means related to IS—which contrasts common findings in other areas of acquisition with comprehension skills typically being in advance to production skills and thus provides a challenge for the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 437 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Despite the overwhelmingly frequent occurrence of focus in utterances, a large number of acquisition studies show that children’s difficulties both with producing and especially with comprehending sentences with focus are manifold even at around 5 or 6 years of age (see Höhle et al, 2016 for a detailed overview). In the following, we concentrate on challenges that are most relevant to this study, namely those concerning the identification and exhaustive interpretation of focus.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite the overwhelmingly frequent occurrence of focus in utterances, a large number of acquisition studies show that children’s difficulties both with producing and especially with comprehending sentences with focus are manifold even at around 5 or 6 years of age (see Höhle et al, 2016 for a detailed overview). In the following, we concentrate on challenges that are most relevant to this study, namely those concerning the identification and exhaustive interpretation of focus.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children acquire the core components of a language (e.g., phonology, morphology, and syntax) based on experience with the ambient language by the age of four or five years (e.g., Hoff 2009). However, their knowledge of information structure (IS)-adapting the production of language to the appropriate informational needs of the interlocutors and specific speech contexts-tends to lag behind (e.g., Höhle et al 2016). An important dimension of IS or information packaging (Chafe 1976) involves a distinction between "old" or "given" information (recently activated information, e.g., a referent mentioned in previous discourse) versus "new" information (e.g., a referent introduced for the first time) (Birner and Ward 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, children also produce, at an early age, the language-specific devices that adults use to mark information structure, such as prosody, varying referential forms (lexical vs. pronominal forms), and different word orders and construction types, among others (Hoff 2009). Nevertheless, children's ability to use these devices to mark information structure in an adult-like manner develops over a protracted trajectory (Höhle et al 2016) which "may be caused by the fact that producing and understanding linguistic means of encoding. .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%