2014
DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000245
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Insight and Search in Katona’s Five-Square Problem

Abstract: Insights are often productive outcomes of human thinking. We provide a cognitive model that explains insight problem solving by the interplay of problem space search and representational change, whereby the problem space is constrained or relaxed based on the problem representation. By introducing different experimental conditions that either constrained the initial search space or helped solvers to initiate a representational change, we investigated the interplay of problem space search and representational c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
15
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
(51 reference statements)
1
15
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Our results are in contrast with an earlier report showing no effect of an initial hint on the solution ( € Ollinger et al, 2014). € Ollinger and colleagues cued participants by highlighting either the matchstick that has to be moved to decompose the chunk of the central square in Katona's (1940) five-square problem or highlighting the position where this matchstick has to be placed for a successful solution.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our results are in contrast with an earlier report showing no effect of an initial hint on the solution ( € Ollinger et al, 2014). € Ollinger and colleagues cued participants by highlighting either the matchstick that has to be moved to decompose the chunk of the central square in Katona's (1940) five-square problem or highlighting the position where this matchstick has to be placed for a successful solution.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 99%
“…We predicted (a) an initial shift of participants' gaze to the left jar across arm movement conditions (e.g., Kazandjian et al, 2010); (b) that the left jar would be used more often for the initial problem representation and subsequently for producing problem solutions (Werner & Raab, 2013; (c) a small effect of the sensori-motor information on solution preferences ; and (d) a combined effect of jar arrangement and sensori-motor information on solution preferences (cf. € Ollinger, Jones, & Knoblich, 2014).…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We acknowledge the limits of an analysis conducted on only one of the seven problems. However, analyses of a single problem are not uncommon in insight-problem solving studies (e.g., Grant and Spivey, 2003; Kershaw et al, 2013; Öllinger et al, 2013, 2014). Moreover, the results which emerged from this analysis were not meant to be conclusive, our intention was merely to offer some further indications on how training might have modified the direction which the participants' search took.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The training we exposed participants to was not specific to a given problem (as in, for example, Chronicle et al, 2001, Experiment 3; Weisberg and Alba, 1981; Grant and Spivey, 2003; Kershaw and Ohlsson, 2004; Kershaw et al, 2013; Öllinger et al, 2013, 2014) but rather provided advice on how to search for a solution to a set of (spatial) problems and in this sense it resembles meta-cognitive training. However, it differs from other types of domain-specific meta-cognitive training investigated in previous literature (some of which is domain specific, e.g., Walinga et al, 2011; Patrick and Ahmed, 2014; Patrick et al, 2015) in that the participants in the present study were asked to use the “oppositional reasoning” strategy they had been told about in their exploration of the spatial domain.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation