2020
DOI: 10.1101/2020.05.31.126359
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Instantaneous movement-unrelated midbrain activity modifies ongoing eye movements

Abstract: 22At any moment in time, new information is sampled from the environment and interacts with 23 ongoing brain state. Often, such interaction takes place within individual circuits that are 24 capable of both mediating the internally ongoing plan as well as representing exogenous 25 sensory events. Here we investigated how sensory-driven neural activity can be integrated, 26 very often in the same neuron types, into ongoing oculomotor commands for saccades. 27Despite the ballistic nature of saccades, visually-in… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

4
14
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 93 publications
(194 reference statements)
4
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A further implication of the results of SC inactivation on early cue-influenced microsaccade directions ( Figure 3 ) is that one can now attempt to establish a quantitative link between microsaccade endpoint variability and SC cue-induced visual bursts. Specifically, the timing of early cue-congruent microsaccades in Figures 1 – 3 is consistent with the timing of SC visual bursts ( Buonocore et al, 2017 , 2020b ). If eliminating such visual bursts via SC inactivation diminishes the likelihood of cue-congruent microsaccades ( Figure 3 ; Hafed et al, 2013 ), then this might suggest that these cue-congruent microsaccades reflect readout of the SC map under a very specific simultaneity condition: a microsaccade burst in the direction of the appearing cue in the rostral SC region, and a simultaneous visual burst in the periphery at the site representing the cue’s location ( Figure 5A ).…”
Section: The Superior Colliculus (Sc)supporting
confidence: 70%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…A further implication of the results of SC inactivation on early cue-influenced microsaccade directions ( Figure 3 ) is that one can now attempt to establish a quantitative link between microsaccade endpoint variability and SC cue-induced visual bursts. Specifically, the timing of early cue-congruent microsaccades in Figures 1 – 3 is consistent with the timing of SC visual bursts ( Buonocore et al, 2017 , 2020b ). If eliminating such visual bursts via SC inactivation diminishes the likelihood of cue-congruent microsaccades ( Figure 3 ; Hafed et al, 2013 ), then this might suggest that these cue-congruent microsaccades reflect readout of the SC map under a very specific simultaneity condition: a microsaccade burst in the direction of the appearing cue in the rostral SC region, and a simultaneous visual burst in the periphery at the site representing the cue’s location ( Figure 5A ).…”
Section: The Superior Colliculus (Sc)supporting
confidence: 70%
“…If eliminating such visual bursts via SC inactivation diminishes the likelihood of cue-congruent microsaccades ( Figure 3 ; Hafed et al, 2013 ), then this might suggest that these cue-congruent microsaccades reflect readout of the SC map under a very specific simultaneity condition: a microsaccade burst in the direction of the appearing cue in the rostral SC region, and a simultaneous visual burst in the periphery at the site representing the cue’s location ( Figure 5A ). If true, then there should be a measurable number of cue-induced visual spikes that are “injected” onto the SC map (when the cue appears) at the same time as the triggering of microsaccades ( Buonocore et al, 2017 , 2020b ). This should “add” to the triggered movements and alter their size.…”
Section: The Superior Colliculus (Sc)mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations