1997
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.992
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intended and unintended overconsumption of physical, spatial, and temporal resources.

Abstract: T\vo experiments investigated whether people overconsume nonpartitioned physical, spatial, and temporal resources; whether this overconsumption reflects intended or unintended processes; and whether these processes have adverse inferential and behavioral consequences. In Experiment 1, members of large groups overconsumed nonpartitioned resources of all types, especially nonpartitioned temporal resources. Moreover, these overconsumptions stemmed from both intentional motives as well as from an unintentional per… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Early distributive justice research indicated that factors unrelated to principles of distributive fairness (e.g., the degree of personal beneWt from an outcome) can inXuence distributive fairness judgments, especially when there is informational uncertainty in the judgmental context (Herlocker, Allison, Foubert, & Beggan, 1997;Thompson & Loewenstein, 1992). Demonstrating a similar pattern with regard to procedural justice facilitates a coherent understanding of how people generally assess justice and meets justice researchers' calls for greater attention to subjective assessments of fairness, since subjective assessments typically have a greater impact on reactions than do objective characteristics of procedures (e.g., Lind, Kanfer, & Earley, 1990;Shapiro & Brett, 1993).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Early distributive justice research indicated that factors unrelated to principles of distributive fairness (e.g., the degree of personal beneWt from an outcome) can inXuence distributive fairness judgments, especially when there is informational uncertainty in the judgmental context (Herlocker, Allison, Foubert, & Beggan, 1997;Thompson & Loewenstein, 1992). Demonstrating a similar pattern with regard to procedural justice facilitates a coherent understanding of how people generally assess justice and meets justice researchers' calls for greater attention to subjective assessments of fairness, since subjective assessments typically have a greater impact on reactions than do objective characteristics of procedures (e.g., Lind, Kanfer, & Earley, 1990;Shapiro & Brett, 1993).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They accounted for many of the differences they find in the interpersonal exchange of the goods they examine by classifying resources along two dimensions: particularism (i.e., the value of a resource as influenced by the particular persons involved in exchanging it and their relationship to one another) and concreteness (i.e., whether the resources are relatively concrete as opposed to symbolic). Some of the findings of these studies are relevant to our research, for example that individuals evaluating hypothetical transfers tended to prefer equality when distributing tangible goods (as well as love and services) and to split in their preferences over equality and merit when allocating money (Törnblom and Foa 1983; see also Herlocker et al 1997).…”
Section: Empirical Studies Of Distributive Behaviormentioning
confidence: 89%
“…They were assured that it was actual money being distributed, although due to budgetary constraints, only 10 percent of the participants would be randomly selected to receive the money allocated to them. This type of situation, where teammates are not physically present for the allocation task, has been used effectively in past research (Allison, McQueen, & Schaerfl , 1992;Herlocker, Allison, Foubert, & Beggan, 1997;Roch et al, 2000). Further, past research has shown that experimental results do not signifi cantly differ when all versus ten percent of participants are paid (Kahneman et al, 1986).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%