2012
DOI: 10.1007/s10882-012-9281-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intentional Communication of Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Judgments of Different Communication Partners

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
26
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
1
26
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One example in which a nonvocal behavior may not be immediately reinforced and, therefore, result in escape or avoidance of a nonpreferred or aversive stimulus is related to those situations when social partners are unfamiliar with the child and with his or her behavioral repertoire. Thus, they are unable to identify the function of the nonvocal behavior emitted by the child and do not respond to his or her communicative attempts (Matthews-Somerville & Cress, 2005; Meadan, Halle, & Kelly, 2012). When a social partner does not respond immediately to a child’s communicative attempt, the child may persist with the same nonvocal behavior (i.e., extinction burst; Lerman, Iwata, & Wallace, 1999), may engage in inappropriate behavior that was reinforced by social partners in the past (i.e., resurgence; Lieving, Hagopian, Long, & O’Connor, 2004), or may eventually refrain from further interaction because of a history of past communication failures, thereby continuing to be exposed to the aversive situation (i.e., learned helplessness or extinction; Guess, Benson, & Siegel-Causey, 1985; Seligman, 1975).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One example in which a nonvocal behavior may not be immediately reinforced and, therefore, result in escape or avoidance of a nonpreferred or aversive stimulus is related to those situations when social partners are unfamiliar with the child and with his or her behavioral repertoire. Thus, they are unable to identify the function of the nonvocal behavior emitted by the child and do not respond to his or her communicative attempts (Matthews-Somerville & Cress, 2005; Meadan, Halle, & Kelly, 2012). When a social partner does not respond immediately to a child’s communicative attempt, the child may persist with the same nonvocal behavior (i.e., extinction burst; Lerman, Iwata, & Wallace, 1999), may engage in inappropriate behavior that was reinforced by social partners in the past (i.e., resurgence; Lieving, Hagopian, Long, & O’Connor, 2004), or may eventually refrain from further interaction because of a history of past communication failures, thereby continuing to be exposed to the aversive situation (i.e., learned helplessness or extinction; Guess, Benson, & Siegel-Causey, 1985; Seligman, 1975).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, informants can overestimate or underestimate communicative abilities (Meadan et al 2012). This is particularly evident when using stakeholders who are either highly involved with or have little knowledge of learners being tested.…”
Section: Nonstandardized Assessment and Individuals With Severe Disabmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conversely, partners with less knowledge of learners or the potential communicative value of unconventional communication signals may fail to recognize clear attempts to communicate. For example, Meadan et al (2012) found that adults who were familiar with children with autism evaluated the presence and purpose of communicative intent more accurately than adults who were unfamiliar with the children.…”
Section: Nonstandardized Assessment and Individuals With Severe Disabmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, an individual might display subtle behaviours when presented with non-preferred objects or activities (e.g., dropping the object, looking away, and closing his or her eyes). If the teacher or carer does not recognise these signs, then they are perhaps likely to persist in presenting non-preferred objects and activities to the person, which could lead to frustration and increasing unresponsiveness (Atkin & Lorch, 2014;Meadan, Halle, & Kelly, 2012;Greathead et al, 2016). It would therefore seem crucial to design and implement effective methods of increasing our understanding of the forms and functions of behaviours that a person with PMD might have that indicate engagement and interest as well as behaviours that might indicate disinterest (Atkin & Lorch, 2014;Downing & Siegel-Causey, 1988;Green, Reid, Canipe, & Gardner, 1991;Green & Reid, 1996;Keen, Sigafoos & Woodyatt, 2001;Lancioni et al, 2013;Greathead et al, 2016;Porter, Ouvry, Morgan & Downs, 2001;.…”
Section: Profound and Multiple Disabilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For persons with PMD, PLBs might consist of a range of idiosyncratic body movements and gestures, facial expressions, and the tensing or relaxing of muscles (Atkin & Lorch, 2014;Greathead et al, 2016;Keen et al, 2001;Porter et al, 2001). Issues arise when a learner relies upon these behaviours as their primary communication tool as it can be difficult for a listener to recognise the behaviour as a communication attempt, decipher if the change in physical state is meaningful, and whether the behaviour was intentional or simply a reflex (Arthur, 2003;Crais & Ogletree, 2016;Greathead et al, 2016;Keen et al, 2001;Meadan, Halle & Kelly, 2012;Porter et al, 2001;Sigafoos et al, 2006). There is evidence to support the existence of an inverse relationship between the degree of ID [intellectual disability] and the frequency and clarity of PLBs (Yoder, 1987;Yoder & Feagans, 1988).…”
Section: Pre-linguistic Behavioursmentioning
confidence: 99%