2022
DOI: 10.1007/s10071-022-01615-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intentional gestural communication amongst red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus)

Abstract: Apes, human’s closest living relatives, are renowned for their intentional and highly flexible use of gestural communication. In stark contrast, evidence for flexible and intentional gestural communication in monkeys is scarce. Here, we investigated the intentionality and flexibility of spontaneous gesture use in red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus). We applied established methods used in ape gesture research to analyse whether the body acts produced by a total of 17 individuals living in three differen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

4
20
0

Year Published

2024
2024
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 120 publications
4
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Bard (1992) introduced Bates' definition of intentional communication to the study of primate communication, distinguishing intentional communication from other intentional behaviors in orangutan mother-infant interactions. In the same line, several behavioral indices of intentional communication inherited from child developmental studies were then applied to nonhuman gestural studies to assess intentionality (see, for reviews, Byrne et al, 2017;Leavens, Russell, & Hopkins, 2005;Liebal, Waller, Burrows, & Slocombe, 2014c;Schel, Bono, Aychet, Pika, & Lemasson, 2020;Townsend et al, 2017). Townsend et al (2017) recently gathered these indices in a general framework implying three main criteria to attribute intentionality to communicative signals: The signal has to be directed toward a recipient, produced to reach a particular social goal, and has to be followed by a response from the recipient that is consistent with the goal that is deduced for the signaler.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…Bard (1992) introduced Bates' definition of intentional communication to the study of primate communication, distinguishing intentional communication from other intentional behaviors in orangutan mother-infant interactions. In the same line, several behavioral indices of intentional communication inherited from child developmental studies were then applied to nonhuman gestural studies to assess intentionality (see, for reviews, Byrne et al, 2017;Leavens, Russell, & Hopkins, 2005;Liebal, Waller, Burrows, & Slocombe, 2014c;Schel, Bono, Aychet, Pika, & Lemasson, 2020;Townsend et al, 2017). Townsend et al (2017) recently gathered these indices in a general framework implying three main criteria to attribute intentionality to communicative signals: The signal has to be directed toward a recipient, produced to reach a particular social goal, and has to be followed by a response from the recipient that is consistent with the goal that is deduced for the signaler.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A signal is considered as recipient-directed if it is produced in the presence of potential receivers (i.e., "socially used") and physically directed to another individual (Call & Tomasello, 2007;Leavens & Hopkins, 1998;Leavens, Hopkins, & Bard, 1996;Leavens, Hopkins, & Thomas, 2004;Liebal et al, 2014a;Schel et al, 2020;Tomasello et al, 1985;Tomasello, Call, Nagell, Olguin, and Carpenter, 1994). To discriminate socially dependent signals from others, supplementary indicators should be taken into account, such as the fact that the signaler first checked the audience's attentional state before producing the display (Genty et al, 2009;Graham et al, 2017;Hobaiter & Byrne, 2011a;Schel et al, 2020), at least in case of signals that have to be perceived on the visual channel.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations