2024
DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2023.09.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interactive repair and the foundations of language

Mark Dingemanse,
N.J. Enfield
Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2024
2024
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 93 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Seminal work in psycholinguistics and conversation analysis suggests that “language use is fundamentally a joint activity” (Clark, 1994, p. 244; Clark & Schaefer, 1989; Clark & Wilkes-Gibbs, 1986; Schegloff et al, 1977): While speaking and listening are individual actions, they contribute toward joint “problems”—errors in achieving shared understanding—and dealing with these “problems” requires joint management, strategies that can help both prevent and repair glitches in shared understanding (Clark, 1994). Taken together, individuals use different verbal, nonverbal, and paralinguistic cues to coordinate their shared understanding on a turn-by-turn basis, and doing so is a uniquely human ability (Dingemanse & Enfield, 2023).…”
Section: Previous Research On Listening—a Conundrum For Determining A...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Seminal work in psycholinguistics and conversation analysis suggests that “language use is fundamentally a joint activity” (Clark, 1994, p. 244; Clark & Schaefer, 1989; Clark & Wilkes-Gibbs, 1986; Schegloff et al, 1977): While speaking and listening are individual actions, they contribute toward joint “problems”—errors in achieving shared understanding—and dealing with these “problems” requires joint management, strategies that can help both prevent and repair glitches in shared understanding (Clark, 1994). Taken together, individuals use different verbal, nonverbal, and paralinguistic cues to coordinate their shared understanding on a turn-by-turn basis, and doing so is a uniquely human ability (Dingemanse & Enfield, 2023).…”
Section: Previous Research On Listening—a Conundrum For Determining A...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With the increasing availability of sign language corpus data produced in naturalistic settings, there has been an increase in studies examining everyday face-to-face interaction, the primordial setting of human communication [69]. Recent research reveals that human communicative interaction strives for efficiency, clarity, and minimization of breakdown or communicative trouble [57,[96][97][98]. Turn transitions happen at a very rapid rate [99].…”
Section: Explanationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Communicators even start planning their own response while still perceiving the previous turn [100]. Non-manual cues have been reported to largely contribute to this fast dynamic and to ensure progressivity in interaction [52,95,98,[101][102][103][104]. Visual cues appear to be best suited for this, as "the core of social interaction perception is visual" [105, p. 1165].…”
Section: Plos Onementioning
confidence: 99%
“…It cannot, for example, clarify the aim of a request while the request is in progress, or modify its response as it is speaking based on the facial expression of the requester. Both of these are kinds of interactional repair, a phenomenon humans engage in frequently and fluidly in naturalistic conversation (Dingemanse & Enfield, 2024), and which plays a key role in effective communication. In short, the mechanisms of LLMs place limits upon its functional similarity with humans.…”
Section: Multimodality and Interactionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even assuming models may eventually reach human-like functional competence in some pragmatic, gestural, or prosodic areas, the functional capacity for language in humans is not limited to the production of more or less well-formed strings for broadcast transmission. There are key functional elements of language that are not available to or emergent in LLMs, for example, turn taking (e.g., Casillas et al, 2016;Levinson, 2016;Stivers et al, 2009), co-speech gesture and multimodality (e.g., Goldin-Meadow & Brentari, 2017;Kita et al, 2007;Rasenberg et al, 2022), repair (e.g., Dingemanse & Enfield, 2024;Dingemanse et al, 2014Dingemanse et al, , 2015Hayashi, Raymond & Sidnell, 2013), and common ground negotiation (e.g., Brennan & Clark, 1996;Brown-Schmidt & Duff, 2016;Clark & Wilkes-Gibbs, 1986) are all essential parts of natural language in interaction. This also blurs into other aspects of our broader socio-cognitive suite including ostensive inference, perspective taking and joint attention (e.g., Heintz & Scott-Phillips, 2023;Tomasello et al, 2005).…”
Section: Multimodality and Interactionmentioning
confidence: 99%