2015
DOI: 10.1037/a0037438
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interference in joint picture naming.

Abstract: In two experiments we provided evidence for a joint interference effect in picture naming. Participants took longer to name pictures when they believed that their partner concurrently named pictures than when they believed their partner was silent (Experiment 1) or concurrently categorized the pictures as being from the same or from different semantic categories (Experiment 2). However, picture naming latencies were not affected by beliefs about what one's partner said. These findings are consistent with the i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

14
96
5

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(115 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
14
96
5
Order By: Relevance
“…If it were, we should have observed a reliable difference between the conditions even when leaders were unable to observe the followers’ actions. There is evidence that coactors seated in separate rooms can represent each other’s actions (Atmaca, Sebanz, & Knoblich, 2011; Gambi, Van de Cavey, & Pickering, 2015). Therefore, a strong version of the anticipation account would predict that merely knowing whether the partner would respond with a compatible or an incompatible action should influence the leader’s action execution (Pfister, Pfeuffer, & Kunde, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If it were, we should have observed a reliable difference between the conditions even when leaders were unable to observe the followers’ actions. There is evidence that coactors seated in separate rooms can represent each other’s actions (Atmaca, Sebanz, & Knoblich, 2011; Gambi, Van de Cavey, & Pickering, 2015). Therefore, a strong version of the anticipation account would predict that merely knowing whether the partner would respond with a compatible or an incompatible action should influence the leader’s action execution (Pfister, Pfeuffer, & Kunde, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Earlier work by Gambi, van de Cavey, and Pickering (2015) showed that speakers performing a joint naming task represented the partner's task, i.e. whether or not the partner also had to name objects, but not the content of the partner's speech plan.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This suggests that what is ideal for conversation may involve only minimally representing a partner's plan. The current work followed directly from this study, using a simplified version of the Gambi et al (2015) paradigm. As such, we begin by describing their procedure and results in detail.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…One might argue that in the absence of any direct across-speaker effects (i.e., no direct context effect in Experiment 2), we cannot be sure that our participants corepresented their partner's speech. However, this seems unlikely, given the fact that Gambi, van de Cavey, and Pickering (2015) found evidence that merely imagining another person's response interfered with one's own naming in a non-interactive context (participants sat in adjacent soundproof booths and could only peripherally see each other through a small window). If there is evidence for mental corepresentation in situations in which it is irrelevant for one's own actions, as was the case in Gambi et al's (2015) study, we should expect it even more in a real communicative situation, that is, in a situation in which two speakers must coordinate their responses to reach a common communicative goal and need to listen to their partner in order to be able to perform their own task.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%