2009
DOI: 10.1515/mult.2009.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interlanguage request modification: The use of lexical/phrasal downgraders and mitigating supportive moves

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
21
0
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
4
21
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Economidou-Kogetsidis' (2008,2009) and Woodfield & EconomidouKogetsidis' (2010) studies both focused on advanced learners of English. Economidou- Kogetsidis (2008Kogetsidis ( , 2009) used written discourse completion tasks to analyse both external and internal request modifiers of advanced Greek learners of English. Findings from her studies showed that, in comparison to British English native speakers, advanced language learners of English used fewer internal modifiers and showed a rather restricted range of peripheral request devices.…”
Section: Previous Studies On Request Modifiersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Economidou-Kogetsidis' (2008,2009) and Woodfield & EconomidouKogetsidis' (2010) studies both focused on advanced learners of English. Economidou- Kogetsidis (2008Kogetsidis ( , 2009) used written discourse completion tasks to analyse both external and internal request modifiers of advanced Greek learners of English. Findings from her studies showed that, in comparison to British English native speakers, advanced language learners of English used fewer internal modifiers and showed a rather restricted range of peripheral request devices.…”
Section: Previous Studies On Request Modifiersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The current study also confirms that regardless of proficiency level, grounders tended to be the most popular external strategy that learners used to mitigate requests in the L2. It is possible that learners provided a justification for their requests (e.g., Economidou‐Kogetsidis, , ; Schauer, ; Woodfield & Economidou‐Kogetsidis, ) because (1) learners attempted to signal concern for the interlocutor's face through lessening the degree of imposition of their own requests (see also Hassall, ), and (2) the flexibility of the linguistic structure needed for external strategies provided learners with a chance to avoid exposing their lack of pragmalinguistic knowledge (see Woodfield & Economidou‐Kogetsidis, ), although this last explanation may not apply to the advanced learners, who seemed to overuse external modification to the point that their linguistic choices diverged from native speaker norms, a finding that was also noted in Al‐Gahtani and Roever ().…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Originally, the WDCT allowed some rule-delimited spacing for the omitted request, followed by a response. However, since Rose found that "[…] the inclusion of hearer response did not have a significant effect on requests elicited […]'' (Rose, 1992, p. 49), questioning the need of including the hearer's answer, since in real communicative exchanges one usually does not know, beforehand, the hearer's reaction, a number of other studies that followed eliminated the rejoinder and used an open questionnaire (Felix-Brasdefer, 2003;Marti, 2006;Economidou-Kogetsidis, 2009;Woodfield & Economidou-Kogetsidis, 2010;Schalkoski-Dias, 2010;among others). Therefore, we have opted to omit the hearer's answer in our WDCTs.…”
Section: Data Collection and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%