2010
DOI: 10.1177/1087054710365993
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Internal Consistency and Discriminant Validity of a Delay-Discounting Task With an Adult Self-Reported ADHD Sample

Abstract: Findings showed that the delay-discounting task is a promising task for both clinical research and practice as it has strong reliability overall and has moderate discriminant ability for self-reported ADHD in young adults.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
34
1
7

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
2
34
1
7
Order By: Relevance
“…A promising start for studying intentional inhibition in motivationally relevant contexts in individuals with ADHD has been made in research employing delay discounting tasks (Barkley, Edwards, Laneri, Fletcher, & Metevia, 2001;Demurie, Roeyers, Baeyens, & Sonuga-Barke, 2012b;Hurst, Kepley, McCalla, & Livermore, 2011;Paloyelis, Asherson, Mehta, Faraone, & Kuntsi, 2010;Plichta et al, 2009;Scheres et al, 2006;Scheres, Tontsch, Thoeny, & Kaczkurkin, 2010;Wilson, Mitchell, Musser, Schmitt, & Nigg, 2011) or choice delay tasks (Solanto et al, 2001;SonugaBarke, Taylor, Sembi, & Smith, 1992; for a review, see Luman, Oosterlaan, and Sergeant (2005)). These tasks are relevant here, because participants have control over whether or not to control the tendency to act by inhibiting and waiting for the delayed reward.…”
Section: Implications For Developmental Disordersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A promising start for studying intentional inhibition in motivationally relevant contexts in individuals with ADHD has been made in research employing delay discounting tasks (Barkley, Edwards, Laneri, Fletcher, & Metevia, 2001;Demurie, Roeyers, Baeyens, & Sonuga-Barke, 2012b;Hurst, Kepley, McCalla, & Livermore, 2011;Paloyelis, Asherson, Mehta, Faraone, & Kuntsi, 2010;Plichta et al, 2009;Scheres et al, 2006;Scheres, Tontsch, Thoeny, & Kaczkurkin, 2010;Wilson, Mitchell, Musser, Schmitt, & Nigg, 2011) or choice delay tasks (Solanto et al, 2001;SonugaBarke, Taylor, Sembi, & Smith, 1992; for a review, see Luman, Oosterlaan, and Sergeant (2005)). These tasks are relevant here, because participants have control over whether or not to control the tendency to act by inhibiting and waiting for the delayed reward.…”
Section: Implications For Developmental Disordersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous examinations indicate that both children (Dalen, Sonuga-Barke, Hall, & Remington 2004;Solanto et al, 2001) and adults (Hurst, Kepley, McCalla, & Livermore, 2011) with ADHD exhibit deficits on choice-impulsivity tasks, evidenced by more frequent selection of immediate-smaller reinforcement relative to delayed-larger reinforcement. These findings have been reliably demonstrated across various iterations of the prototypical choice-impulsivity task, including the Maudsley Index of Delay Aversion (Kuntsi, Stevenson, Oosterlaan, & Sonuga-Barke, 2001), flower-delay task (Sonuga-Barke et al, 1992), and delay discounting tasks (Rachlin, Raineri, & Cross, 1991;Hurst et al, 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Also, on delay of gratification tasks (Mischel et al, 1989) children with hyperactivity exhibit a greater tendency to terminate the delay to obtain a smaller reward, rather than waiting an allotted time for a larger reward (Rapport et al, 1986). Furthermore, several studies now report steeper monetary discounting in children with ADHD (Paloyelis et al, 2009;Wilson et al, 2011;Demurie et al, 2012) or in adults with previous ADHD (Hurst et al, 2011).…”
Section: Adhd As a Deficit Statementioning
confidence: 99%