1996
DOI: 10.1007/bf03172944
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Internality, academic status and intergroup attributions

Abstract: Some pupils categorized as good vs. bad pupils were given a questionnaire of attributions, thus allowing the calculation of internality scores. They were also requested to answer as would a good (vs. a bad) pupil do. Finally, they also had to predict in which way a good (vs. a bad) pupil would answer on their behalf. The results were explained within the frame of two different theoretical fields: the theory of the norm of internality and the biases in terms of intergroup attributions. These results show that 1… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
15
0
2

Year Published

1998
1998
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
2
15
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Namely, high-status group members almost systematically favor their own group and sometimes derogate the outgroup, whereas low-status group members often exhibit outgroup favoritism, i.e., favoring the high-status group (Brown, 1995, for review;Guimond, Dif, & Aupy, 2002). On this basis, authors (Dubois & Beauvois, 1996;Dubois, Beauvois, Gilibert, & Zentner, 2000;Pansu et al, 2005) have considered that, as people tend to attribute more positive judgments to their ingroup in equivalent intergroup relations, then a judgment systematically attributed to an ingroup should be considered socially valued. Similarly, as people tend to attribute more positive judgments to a high-status group in asymmetric intergroup relations, then a judgment systematically attributed to the high-status group should be considered socially valued.…”
Section: Intergroup Biases As Indicators Of the Social Valorization Omentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Namely, high-status group members almost systematically favor their own group and sometimes derogate the outgroup, whereas low-status group members often exhibit outgroup favoritism, i.e., favoring the high-status group (Brown, 1995, for review;Guimond, Dif, & Aupy, 2002). On this basis, authors (Dubois & Beauvois, 1996;Dubois, Beauvois, Gilibert, & Zentner, 2000;Pansu et al, 2005) have considered that, as people tend to attribute more positive judgments to their ingroup in equivalent intergroup relations, then a judgment systematically attributed to an ingroup should be considered socially valued. Similarly, as people tend to attribute more positive judgments to a high-status group in asymmetric intergroup relations, then a judgment systematically attributed to the high-status group should be considered socially valued.…”
Section: Intergroup Biases As Indicators Of the Social Valorization Omentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, as people tend to attribute more positive judgments to a high-status group in asymmetric intergroup relations, then a judgment systematically attributed to the high-status group should be considered socially valued. For example, as it is socially valued, internality is systematically attributed to the ingroup in studies activating equivalent intergroup relations (Dubois et al, 2000) and to the high statusgroup in studies based on asymmetric intergroup relations (Dubois & Beauvois, 1996;Beauvois, Gilibert, Pansu, & Abdelaoui, 1998;Pansu et al, 2005).…”
Section: Intergroup Biases As Indicators Of the Social Valorization Omentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After choosing their own causal explanations, the subjects have to choose those of other persons who are assumed to be valued to different extents. The results obtained with this paradigm show that subjects often apply the internality strategy to describe persons or groups that carry value (Dubois & Beauvois, 1996;Pansu, Tarquinio, & Gilibert, 2005). In the judge paradigm, subjects must judge persons who are known by their explanation strategy: internality, externality, self-serving or modesty.…”
Section: Social Norm Of Internality: Positive Value Of Internal Explamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, people who endorse negative in-group stereotypes for their own benefit are likely to be viewed negatively by others. In general, people unfavorably evaluate those who make excuses and favorably evaluate individuals who accept personal responsibility for their performance outcomes (Beauvois & Dubois, 1988;Dubois & Beauvois, 1996;Hirt, McCrea, & Boris, 2003). In this regard, when people use the strategy of endorsing negative in-group stereotypes, they may be viewed as selfdefeating and disloyal.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%