1999
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1999.72-317
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interresponse‐time Sensitivity During Discrete‐trial and Free‐operant Concurrent Variable‐interval Schedules

Abstract: Two experiments investigated the sensitivity of pigeons' choice to elapsed time since the last response (i.e., to inter-response time [IRT]) during concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedules. Experiment 1 used a two-key discrete-trial procedure with variable intertrial intervals. Experiment 2 employed a three-key free-operant procedure. In both experiments choice was found to be a function of the active-schedule IRT, defined as the time since the most recent response. Monte Carlo simulations show… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
43
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
(68 reference statements)
0
43
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Variations in the qualities of IRTs across different reinforcement schedules provide summaries of the variability of response timing similar to those revealed by cumulative records of responses. Measures of IRTs can be easily extracted from records of response times, and the shape of the histogram created from these measures can often be adequately described using simple mathematical functions, such as the exponential function (e.g., Sidman, 1954), Markov chains (Cleaveland, 1999), or the log survivor plot (Shull et al, 2001). Additional measures are required to characterize sequential temporal structure across IRTs.…”
Section: Analyzing Temporal Regularities In Response Patternsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Variations in the qualities of IRTs across different reinforcement schedules provide summaries of the variability of response timing similar to those revealed by cumulative records of responses. Measures of IRTs can be easily extracted from records of response times, and the shape of the histogram created from these measures can often be adequately described using simple mathematical functions, such as the exponential function (e.g., Sidman, 1954), Markov chains (Cleaveland, 1999), or the log survivor plot (Shull et al, 2001). Additional measures are required to characterize sequential temporal structure across IRTs.…”
Section: Analyzing Temporal Regularities In Response Patternsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although no existing models of choice behavior, or of operant conditioning more generally, are sufficiently detailed to make specific predictions about how rats' response timing should vary as a function of experience with complex choice tasks such as the one used in the current study, models that take into account the timing of individual responses can potentially provide insights into the possible sources of the observed patterns of responding (Cerutti & Staddon, 2004;Cleaveland, 1999;Misak & Cleaveland, 2011;Shimp, 1969Shimp, , 1981. For example, Staddon's linear waiting model of responding in choice tasks proposes that the interval between recent reinforcement deliveries can strongly modulate the timing of an individual's responses (Staddon, Chelaru, & Higa, 2002;Wynne, Staddon, & Delius, 1996).…”
Section: Relevance To Understanding Matching Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The contingencies of reinforcement for a VI schedule, then, explicitly target t i , the inter‐esponse time (IRT) that separates choices at a particular schedule. In concurrent VI VI schedules every choice can be defined in relation to two such IRTs: active time and background time (Cleaveland, 1999). Active time corresponds to the time since the most recent schedule choice, while background time refers to the time since the alternative schedule was chosen.…”
Section: Theoretical Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This time variable has been termed “active time,” and the model describing its relevance to choice behavior has been designated the active time model, or ATM (Cleaveland, 1999). ATM is a stochastic, molecular model that successfully describes a range of choice behavior for pigeons responding on concurrent VI VI schedules of reinforcement (Brown & Cleaveland, 2009; Cleaveland 1999, 2008; McKenzie & Cleaveland, 2010). The model assumes that during training pigeons learn a function that relates active times to switches and stays into and out of choice “states.” With its emphasis on interresponse times and switches versus stays, ATM falls within a broad theoretical approach to choice behavior that is shared by models such as momentary maximization (Shimp, 1969) and the stay/switch model (MacDonall, 2009).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is possible in this commentary to provide only the merest hint of the full range of views on the difference between local and global analyses. To gain greater perspective, in order to reduce possible distorting effects of just my view, a reader might wish to consult diverse examples, a few of which include Baum (1994Baum ( , 1995Baum ( , 2001Baum ( , 2002Cleaveland (1999); Dinsmoor (2001); Hawkes and Shimp (1998); Herrnstein (1961Herrnstein ( , 1970; Herrnstein and Vaughan (1980); Heyman and Tanz (1995); Hineline (2001); Hineline, Silberberg, Ziriax, Timberlake, and Vaughan (1987); Staddon (1983a, 1983b); Horner (2002) ;Horner, Staddon, and Lozano (1997);MacDonall (1998MacDonall ( , 2000; Nevin (1969); Peele, Casey, and Silberberg (1984); Rachlin (1994Rachlin ( , 2000; Rachlin and Laibson (1997) ;Reed, Soh, Hildebrandt, DeJongh, and Shek, (2000); Reid, Chadwick, Duhham, and Miller (2001); Shimp (1966Shimp ( , 1976bShimp ( , 1992; Silberberg and Ziriax (1985); Silva, Pear, Tait, and Forest (1996); Staddon (1964Staddon ( , 1968Staddon ( , 2001Vaughan (1981); Wearden and Clark (1989);…”
Section: Features Of Local and Global Analyses: Do They Define A Paramentioning
confidence: 99%