2017
DOI: 10.1177/0075424217733026
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Inversion after Clause-Initial Adverbs in Old English: The Special Status ofþa,þonne,nu, andswa

Abstract: This study is a corpus-based analysis of clause-initial adverbs and their ability to invert pronominal and nominal subjects in Old English (OE) prose. There is a limited set of adverbs, referred to as “operators” in generative studies of OE syntax, which may cause inversion of personal pronoun subjects; these are þa, þonne, nu, and swa. In this study, numerous differences between the syntactic behavior of these adverbs are revealed, showing that they should not be treated as a syntactically coherent group. The… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

3
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…( The choice between XVS and XSV for nominal subjects depends on information structure; new subjects follow the XVS pattern more readily (Bech 2001: 163), but for a personal pronoun subject to appear in the XVS pattern, as in (5), the clauseinitial element must belong to a small and closed set of constituents: wh-words, the negative particle ne or one of the adverbs-operators, namely þa 'then' , þonne 'then' and, with some limitations, nu 'now' and swa 'so' (Ringe & Taylor 2014: 399;Cichosz 2017c). One generative interpretation of this syntactic peculiarity is to assume the existence of two parallel types of V2 in OE: operator fronting (V-to-C movement), where the verb moves to a higher position in the clause, triggering categorical inversion of all subject types as in (2) and ( 5), and non-operator fronting (Vto-I movement), where the verb is lower in the clause structure and its position remains sensitive to information structure, as in (4) (Ringe & Taylor 2014: 399).…”
Section: Formmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…( The choice between XVS and XSV for nominal subjects depends on information structure; new subjects follow the XVS pattern more readily (Bech 2001: 163), but for a personal pronoun subject to appear in the XVS pattern, as in (5), the clauseinitial element must belong to a small and closed set of constituents: wh-words, the negative particle ne or one of the adverbs-operators, namely þa 'then' , þonne 'then' and, with some limitations, nu 'now' and swa 'so' (Ringe & Taylor 2014: 399;Cichosz 2017c). One generative interpretation of this syntactic peculiarity is to assume the existence of two parallel types of V2 in OE: operator fronting (V-to-C movement), where the verb moves to a higher position in the clause, triggering categorical inversion of all subject types as in (2) and ( 5), and non-operator fronting (Vto-I movement), where the verb is lower in the clause structure and its position remains sensitive to information structure, as in (4) (Ringe & Taylor 2014: 399).…”
Section: Formmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some speakers may have analysed such sequences as a part of the V-2 construction, which in OE involved inversion of nominal subjects and non-inversion of subject pronouns (Fischer et al 2000;Ringe & Taylor 2014), so clauses such as (64) and (65), as well as all the parenthetical reporting clauses with secgan, are consistent with it. However, reporting clauses with inverted pronominal subjects such as (66) do not pattern with OE V-2 clauses because according to the OE V-2 rule only a closed group of short adverbs, especially þa 'then' and þonne 'then', as well as the negative particle ne, could cause inversion of subject pronouns (Pintzuk 1999: 91;Cichosz 2017a It is difficult to argue that the quoted message could form a coherent group with these lexical elements (treated as 'operators' in formal accounts of OE syntax), especially given that the inversion mechanism is not consistently applied in all the texts. What is more, Bech (2001) shows that inversion after 'non-operators' is more common when nominal subjects are new; given subjects usually stay non-inverted, so the fact that 100 per cent of nominal subjects in OE reporting clauses are inverted is also quite unexpected from the point of view of the OE V-2 because the speakers referred to in reporting clauses are rarely discourse-new.…”
Section: The Development Of the Parenthetical Reporting Constructionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, reporting clauses with inverted pronominal subjects such as (66) do not pattern with OE V-2 clauses because according to the OE V-2 rule only a closed group of short adverbs, especially þa ‘then’ and þonne ‘then’, as well as the negative particle ne , could cause inversion of subject pronouns (Pintzuk 1999: 91; Cichosz 2017a). …”
Section: The Development Of the Parenthetical Reporting Constructionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“… 2 Some studies also include nu (Haeberli 2002; van Kemenade & Los 2009; Ringe & Taylor 2014) and swa (Ringe & Taylor 2014) in this set; see Cichosz (2017c) for a comprehensive analysis of their syntactic behavior. …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%