2018
DOI: 10.1111/lcrp.12127
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Investigating deception in second language speakers: Interviewee and assessor perspectives

Abstract: Purpose. The first of two experiments investigated the effect that speaking in a nonnative language has on interviewees' perceptions of their interview experience. A second experiment investigated evaluators' perceptions of the credibility of interviewees who spoke in their native or non-native language.Method. For the first experiment, 52 participants told the truth or lied about their identity during a mock border control interview. All of the participants were interviewed in English, for half of the sample … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
28
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
(112 reference statements)
4
28
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Eyewitnesses who give their statement in a non‐native language often need to exert more effort than native speakers. In a recent study by Akehurst, Arnhold, Figueiredo, Turtle, and Leach (), participants interviewed in a non‐native language in a mock border patrol interview reported feeling more nervous and cognitively strained compared with participants interviewed in their native language. Abutalebi (), summing up past research on the production and control of a second language, stated that one explanation of this language effect is that lexical terms in the native and non‐native language compete with each other.…”
Section: Cognitive Load For Native Vs Non‐native Speaking Witnessesmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Eyewitnesses who give their statement in a non‐native language often need to exert more effort than native speakers. In a recent study by Akehurst, Arnhold, Figueiredo, Turtle, and Leach (), participants interviewed in a non‐native language in a mock border patrol interview reported feeling more nervous and cognitively strained compared with participants interviewed in their native language. Abutalebi (), summing up past research on the production and control of a second language, stated that one explanation of this language effect is that lexical terms in the native and non‐native language compete with each other.…”
Section: Cognitive Load For Native Vs Non‐native Speaking Witnessesmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…When expectations about how a speaker should behave are violated, suspicion can be aroused (e.g., Bond et al, ). Non‐native speakers report and overtly display signs of nervousness and cognitive demands during interviews (e.g., speech hesitations and phrase repetition; Akehurst et al, ; Elliott & Leach, ; Gregersen, ). Thus, observers' negative biases when judging non‐native speakers (e.g., Da Silva & Leach, ; Evans & Michael, ) could be explained by this failure to adjust expectations in keeping with speakers' language proficiencies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The same consideration does not appear to be afforded to non‐native speakers, however. Across several studies, non‐native speakers are viewed less positively than native speakers, either because they are less likely to be labeled truth‐tellers or they tend to be labeled lie‐tellers (e.g., Akehurst et al, ; Da Silva & Leach, ; Evans & Michael, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations