2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.12.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Investigating sustained attention ability in the elderly by using two different approaches: Inhibiting ongoing behavior versus responding on rare occasions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
63
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(71 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
8
63
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, following the transition zone the results show a consistent increase in post error slowing of 3.5ms per year (95%CI: 3.2 to 3.7). This result suggests a gradual shift towards greater error monitoring across the lifespan above and beyond that explained by slower reaction times due to changes in central nervous functioning in older adults (Ratcliff, et al, 2001), and is consistent with previous self-reports by older participants of less mind wandering during task completion and greater intrinsic motivation to perform well on similar tasks (Staub, et al, 2014). Collectively, then, the mean reaction time, criterion, and post-error slowing measurements all show a monotonic trend across participants 15 years and older representing a strategic shift toward a slower, more cautious approach to the task that diverges significantly from the pattern observed in the d’ and reaction time variability measures.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…However, following the transition zone the results show a consistent increase in post error slowing of 3.5ms per year (95%CI: 3.2 to 3.7). This result suggests a gradual shift towards greater error monitoring across the lifespan above and beyond that explained by slower reaction times due to changes in central nervous functioning in older adults (Ratcliff, et al, 2001), and is consistent with previous self-reports by older participants of less mind wandering during task completion and greater intrinsic motivation to perform well on similar tasks (Staub, et al, 2014). Collectively, then, the mean reaction time, criterion, and post-error slowing measurements all show a monotonic trend across participants 15 years and older representing a strategic shift toward a slower, more cautious approach to the task that diverges significantly from the pattern observed in the d’ and reaction time variability measures.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…These results are in line with those of recent studies using shorter Go/No-Go sustained attention tasks showing that older adults were generally more accurate than their younger counterparts both overall and with time on task (Brache et al, 2010;Carriere et al, 2010;Jackson & Balota, 2012;McVay et al, 2013;Staub et al, 2014;but see McAvinue et al, 2012). Surprisingly, even though the instructions emphasized ac curacy over speed, older participants responded faster than younger subjects, and maintained their speed throughout the watch-keeping period, whereas young subjects became faster with time on task.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…Because young and older adults may differ in this ability, self-caught designs have rarely been used in the aging and MW literature, and when they have, self-caught methods have been combined with probe-caught methods (Giambra, 1989; Jackson & Balota, 2012; Jackson et al, 2013). Finally, a third method that has been used in the MW and aging literature involves retrospective questionnaires that ask subjects to report the frequency with which they experienced certain thoughts following the completion of an experimental task (Lachman & Agrigoroaei, 2012; Maillet & Rajah, 2013; Staub et al, 2014). The main advantage of using retrospective questionnaires is that doing so leaves performance of the task undisturbed.…”
Section: Mind-wanderingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, interest in the general topic of spontaneous cognition has increased rapidly during recent years in both psychology and neuroscience (e.g., Andrews-Hanna et al, 2010; Christoff et al, 2011; O'Callaghan et al, 2015; Smallwood & Schooler, 2015), and the trend seems likely to continue. Second, the emergence of studies on MW in older adults during the past few years (e.g., Jackson & Balota, 2012; Krawietz et al, 2012; Maillet & Rajah, 2013; Nagamatsu et al, 2013; Staub et al, 2014) suggests an increasing focus on spontaneous cognition among cognitive aging researchers. Third, although studies of different spontaneous thought types are typically pursued independently of one another, we think that it is useful to consider them together in order to encourage questions and future investigations about similarities and differences among these processes as a function of aging.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%