2015
DOI: 10.1007/s40750-015-0036-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is Kinship a Schema? Moral Decisions and the Function of the Human Kin Naming System

Abstract: The human kinship system, and its associated terminology, bears the hallmarks of an evolutionary adaptation but its evolutionary origins have not been explored. We argue that the human kinship naming system is a schema that evolved to reduce the cognitive load of maintaining kinships, allowing the expansion of the human network and an increase in survival. We report on the results of two response time studies, using moral dilemmas as a proxy for relationship maintenance, which test the hypothesis. We find qual… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
7
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
(76 reference statements)
2
7
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In line with previous research on the social processing of kin and non-kin groups (Brashears, 2013;Machin & Dunbar, 2016;Wlodarski & Dunbar, 2016), we found divergent effects for different group types: While non-kin groups were perceived as more cohesive when they were smaller, this was not the case for kin groups. This finding provides further support for the notion that kin groups are processed and perceived differently from non-kin groups.…”
Section: Theoretical Implicationssupporting
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In line with previous research on the social processing of kin and non-kin groups (Brashears, 2013;Machin & Dunbar, 2016;Wlodarski & Dunbar, 2016), we found divergent effects for different group types: While non-kin groups were perceived as more cohesive when they were smaller, this was not the case for kin groups. This finding provides further support for the notion that kin groups are processed and perceived differently from non-kin groups.…”
Section: Theoretical Implicationssupporting
confidence: 89%
“…The kinship label was shown to serve as a compression heuristic when recalling social network information, because it contains rich additional information on how people are connected to one another (Brashears, 2013). This is further supported by evidence showing that kin ties are processed faster than non-kin ties (Machin & Dunbar, 2016), and that kin ties are processed in other brain regions than non-kin ties (Wlodarski & Dunbar, 2016). To account for this, we ran additional analyses separating kin and non-kin groups.…”
Section: Ancillary Analysesmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Related research shows that memory for social networks is fundamentally different than memory for identically structured, but non-social, networks, demonstrating the need to study social memory in particular 15 , 16 . Compression heuristics have also been shown to influence moral dilemma decision time 17 , and are consistent with fMRI studies showing differential recruitment of brain regions in the processing of kin and non-kin social ties 18 . We build on this research by using a mechanistic model to investigate the algorithmic processes employed by the brain to recall social information.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 76%
“…For instance, when and how does the concept BROTHER become part of a child's concept of UNCLE? Machin & Dunbar (2016) propose that kinship terminologies may have evolved to reduce the cognitive load of maintaining social relationships. If kinship terms are indeed a cognitive tool that helps us navigate our social world, what are the developmental processes by which children acquire this valuable linguistic knowledge?…”
Section: 2mentioning
confidence: 99%