“…This a priori focus on an “all is a matter of statistical properties” could not be the sole and unique approach to account for these syllable-based effects. Questions come from controversies—and inconsistencies—in studies that banked on quantifiable orthographic statistical—and distributional—properties, like the bigram trough ( Seidenberg, 1987 ), either in children (e.g., in French, Kandel et al, 2011 ; Doignon-Camus et al, 2013 ; Doignon-Camus and Zagar, 2014 ; Maïonchi-Pino et al, 2015a ) or in adults (e.g., in French, Doignon and Zagar, 2005 , 2006 ; Doignon-Camus et al, 2009b ; in Spanish, Carreiras et al, 1993 ; Conrad et al, 2009 ; in English, Rapp, 1992 ; Muncer et al, 2014 ). The clincher of the bigram trough hypothesis to account for mapping letter clusters that frequently co-occur onto syllables and define perceptual syllable boundaries (i.e., “AN.VIL”; e.g., Seidenberg, 1987 ; Doignon-Camus et al, 2009a , b , 2013 ) is that letter co-occurrences straddling syllable boundaries (e.g., the bigram “NV” in “ANVIL”) are of lower frequency than letter co-occurrences preceding or after syllable boundaries (i.e., “AN” and “VI”).…”