2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2005.10.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Isocaloric meal and snack foods differentially affect eating behavior

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
26
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
1
26
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Perceiving the intake between the three main meals as meals rather than snacks has consequences over nutritional quality (Keller et al, 2014) and over the quantity consumed in the meals which follow, and may compensate for meals skipped, such as breakfast or lunch (Capaldi, Quinn, & Privitera, 2006;Wansink et al, 2010). The forenoon and afternoon meal are MIBs that are clearly underexplored, as studies about meal patterns do not usually differentiate within the meals or they take into account only the three main meals (Mesas et al, 2012), coding the rest of the eating occasions as snacking.…”
Section: Having a Forenoon Mealmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Perceiving the intake between the three main meals as meals rather than snacks has consequences over nutritional quality (Keller et al, 2014) and over the quantity consumed in the meals which follow, and may compensate for meals skipped, such as breakfast or lunch (Capaldi, Quinn, & Privitera, 2006;Wansink et al, 2010). The forenoon and afternoon meal are MIBs that are clearly underexplored, as studies about meal patterns do not usually differentiate within the meals or they take into account only the three main meals (Mesas et al, 2012), coding the rest of the eating occasions as snacking.…”
Section: Having a Forenoon Mealmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the other hand, meal-replacement ‘shakes’ are drank like a beverage but marketed and consumed as a ‘meal’ rather than as a ‘drink’, and when consumed in this context have been shown to promote weight loss [32]. Indeed, experimental studies indicate that satiety-related beliefs are important for appetite control [33][35]. For example, participants ate more at a test meal after consuming a food perceived to be a snack compared to participants who consumed the same food but believed it to be a meal [33].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, experimental studies indicate that satiety-related beliefs are important for appetite control [33][35]. For example, participants ate more at a test meal after consuming a food perceived to be a snack compared to participants who consumed the same food but believed it to be a meal [33]. This may be because a meal is associated with greater satiety and so foods consumed in this context are expected to be more satiating than the same foods consumed as a snack.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such perceptions can influence food selection (Steptoe, Pollard, & Wardle, 1995) and energy intake (Buckland, Graham Finlayson, & Hetherington, 2013;Capaldi, Owens, & Privitera, 2006;Provencher, Polivy, & Herman, 2009). For instance, consuming preloads perceived as meals subsequent test meal intake compared to consuming preloads perceived as snacks (Capaldi et al, 2006;Pliner & Zec, 2007). Perceptions about foods can also reduce food intake.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%