Color Categories in Thought and Language 1997
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9780511519819.014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

It's not really red, green, yellow, blue: an inquiry into perceptual color space

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
137
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 153 publications
(147 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
10
137
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, subsequent investigation of the retinogeniculate cone-opponent pathways of the visual system that underpin the early encoding of color has revealed that these pathways do not encode the "basic" white, black, red, green, blue, and yellow categories (and the perceptually pure "unique" hues of these categories) as originally proposed (32). What are commonly known as the "red-green" and "blue-yellow" cardinal cone-opponent mechanisms are actually better described as "cherry-teal" and "chartreuse-violet" in terms of the appearance of the colors they encode.…”
Section: Significancementioning
confidence: 98%
“…However, subsequent investigation of the retinogeniculate cone-opponent pathways of the visual system that underpin the early encoding of color has revealed that these pathways do not encode the "basic" white, black, red, green, blue, and yellow categories (and the perceptually pure "unique" hues of these categories) as originally proposed (32). What are commonly known as the "red-green" and "blue-yellow" cardinal cone-opponent mechanisms are actually better described as "cherry-teal" and "chartreuse-violet" in terms of the appearance of the colors they encode.…”
Section: Significancementioning
confidence: 98%
“…Following the tradition established by Hering, and later by Jameson and Hurvich [2], when color opponent cells were discovered in the primate lateral geniculate nucleus, they were described as being selective for red and green, and blue and yellow [3]. Though it is now clear that the chromatic tuning of color-opponent mechanisms revealed psychophysically does not map on to the Hering primaries [4], and that the color-sensitive cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus encode intermediate color directions [5][6], the conviction that unique hues must have prominence in neural color signals has led to persistence of the idea that coloropponent cells encode the Hering primaries, and to a search for higher-order color mechanisms that are selective for red, green, blue and yellow [7][8][9][10].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More sophisticated physiological models will probably not alter that fact. Indeed, the results hint why it has not been possible to explain basic colour categories based on physiological constraints alone (e.g., Gellatly 1995;Jameson & D'Andrade 1997;Saunders & van Brakel 1997). If different populations exposed to different environmental stimuli and ecological challenges were to be compared, the repertoires of the agents in the population would be even more different.…”
Section: Individualistic Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…20), providing natural anchors for color categories. See also the Interpoint Distance Model ( Jameson & D'Andrade 1997). If such ultimately physiological sources of structure can be operationalized for testing with S&B's methodology, combined with a realistic color environment and coupling among agents, it may be that they lead to convergence.…”
Section: David Bimlermentioning
confidence: 99%