1986
DOI: 10.1007/bf02382594
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) social development: Sex differences in Juvenile behavior

Abstract: ABSTRACT. We have quantitatively documented the development of sex differences in the behavior of juvenile Japanese macaques(1 to 2 years of age). Mothers treated their offspring differently by sex, i.e., mothers of males broke contact with them more frequently than did mothers of females. Juvenile males played more, and mounted other macaques more frequently; juvenile females groomed their mothers more and were also punished by other group members more frequently than were males. Males showed a pattern of dec… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
34
0

Year Published

1993
1993
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
5
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast to our prediction, males were as aggressive as females toward maternal kin throughout development, although only females remain in their natal group and have a higher chance to compete with maternal kin over the same resources. These results support the suggestion that being aggressive might largely serve a similar function for both sexes during the first years of development, and juvenile aggressive behavior does not necessarily anticipate adult patterns (Cords et al 2010;Eaton et al 1986;Raleigh et al 1979;Wolfheim 1977). Whereas affiliative behavior allows individuals to construct lasting relationships over long time frames (Kulik et al 2015), aggressive behavior might serve more immediate functions, i.e., to solve contingent conflictual situations, and thus fail to completely anticipate future patterns of interactions.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In contrast to our prediction, males were as aggressive as females toward maternal kin throughout development, although only females remain in their natal group and have a higher chance to compete with maternal kin over the same resources. These results support the suggestion that being aggressive might largely serve a similar function for both sexes during the first years of development, and juvenile aggressive behavior does not necessarily anticipate adult patterns (Cords et al 2010;Eaton et al 1986;Raleigh et al 1979;Wolfheim 1977). Whereas affiliative behavior allows individuals to construct lasting relationships over long time frames (Kulik et al 2015), aggressive behavior might serve more immediate functions, i.e., to solve contingent conflictual situations, and thus fail to completely anticipate future patterns of interactions.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…As the third variable in the threeway interactions, we included 1) partner's sex, 2) age difference between subject and partner, 3) partner's rank, 4) subject's rank, 5) kin relation between subject and partner, or 6) mother's presence, which was included as a control and not interpreted (cf. Mundry 2014), as each of these variables might affect aggressive behavior in primates, e.g., rank (Lambert 2005;Pereira and Kappeler 1997;Silk et al 1981), sex (Dittus 1977(Dittus , 1979Eaton et al 1986;Silk et al 1981), age (Bernstein andEhardt 1985c, 1986;Campbell 2006;Valero et al 2006;Widdig et al 2001), and kin (Bernstein and Ehardt 1986;Glick et al 1986;Janus 1991a;Widdig et al 2002). To achieve a valid model we also included all the two-way interactions covered by these interactions.…”
Section: Data Analysesmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The fact that sex differences are rather consistent across species and partially emerge early on during infancy (Glick et al 1986; Lonsdorf et al 2014; Milton 2002; Nakamichi 1989; cf . Eaton et al 1986) might suggest that they are not flexible, but preponderantly genetically encoded, or that environmental effects act prenatally or very early in life (Cords et al 2010; Lonsdorf et al 2014; Roney and Maestripieri 2005). For example, young male spider monkeys developed species-typical social patterns despite any male model being present in the isolated population studied, which suggests intrinsic sex differences in social behavior (Milton 2002; for a similar conclusion, see Eaton et al 1986; Roney and Maestriperi 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Male and female macaque infants receive differential treatment early in life both from their own mothers and from other group members, and they in turn treat their mothers and other group members differently [12,24,31,41], Mothers restrain, retrieve, groom and maintain contact more with female infants than with males (rhesus macaque [6]; Japa nese macaque [41,42]). Female infants of Tibetan macaques were carried and groomed by their mothers more than male infants [25].…”
Section: Sexmentioning
confidence: 99%