2008
DOI: 10.1080/09540090802091958
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Joint attention and language evolution

Abstract: This study investigates how more advanced joint attentional mechanisms, rather than only shared attention between two agents and an object, can be implemented and how they influence the results of language games played by these agents. We present computer simulations with language games showing that adding constructs that mimic the three stages of joint attention identified in children's early development (checking attention, following attention and directing attention) substantially increase the performance o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Two of the three joint attentional mechanisms proposed in [29] were implemented: checking/sharing attention and following attention. 4 Checking/sharing attention is the most basic one and is initiated by the agent that becomes the speaker.…”
Section: Dialogue Control and Joint Attentionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two of the three joint attentional mechanisms proposed in [29] were implemented: checking/sharing attention and following attention. 4 Checking/sharing attention is the most basic one and is initiated by the agent that becomes the speaker.…”
Section: Dialogue Control and Joint Attentionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Autistic children having low levels of JA also exhibit poor communicative or linguistic abilities [5], and improving their levels of JA via early interference or intensive training can enhance their expressive language abilities [6,7]. Some simulation studies [8] also demonstrate that when artificial agents are learning word meanings, a high level of JA greatly reduces the number of candidate meanings for target words, thus assisting language acquisition. Meanwhile, comparative evidence reveals significant differences in the level of JA between humans and non-human primates: 9-12 month old human infants can easily establish common ground during interactive or collaborative activities [9], whereas the level of JA in wild/captive non-human primates of different ages is comparatively low, in particular, collaborative intentions are not easy to share among non-human primates or between human experimenters and captive primates [10].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Focusing mainly on normal/autistic children and word learning, available acquisition studies [15] fail to reveal the JAlanguage correlation at a social scale and the effect of JA on general language learning in normal children/adults. Moreover, in those simulation studies [8], JA comes to help interpret linguistic utterances only if linguistic knowledge fails to do so, and listeners get direct feedback that clarify speakers' intended meanings. Such 'mind-reading' simplification [16] cannot trace when people start relying on linguistic knowledge and expressions, instead of nonlinguistic cues, in comprehension.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In Kwisthout et al (2008) we took up Tomasello's (1999) suggestion that the ability to engage in various forms of joint attention provides an essential mechanism for, and may have co-evolved with, cultural learning and language. In checking attention, participants (e.g., a child and parent) consider what the other is focusing his/her attention on (establishing what the other is looking at, for instance).…”
Section: Social Interaction and Sharing A Vocabularymentioning
confidence: 99%