2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.forsciint.2020.110433
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Juror appraisals of forensic evidence: Effects of blind proficiency and cross-examination

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
19
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

3
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
0
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The most common way to counter forensic evidence is through cross-examination, but efforts to educate jurors on the limits of forensic evidence using cross-examination have shown mixed effects (e.g., Crozier, Kukucka, & Garrett, 2020;Eastwood & Caldwell, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The most common way to counter forensic evidence is through cross-examination, but efforts to educate jurors on the limits of forensic evidence using cross-examination have shown mixed effects (e.g., Crozier, Kukucka, & Garrett, 2020;Eastwood & Caldwell, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The most common way to counter forensic evidence is through cross‐examination, but efforts to educate jurors on the limits of forensic evidence using cross‐examination have shown mixed effects (e.g., Crozier, Kukucka, & Garrett, 2020; Eastwood & Caldwell, 2015). In our study, the rebuttal expert's methodological critique was more effective than a methodological critique undertaken through cross‐examination alone (Koehler et al, 2016), and the use of a rebuttal expert permitted the defense not only to effectively raise concerns about the opposing expert's method but also to put on contradictory opinions about the source of the latent print recovered from the crime scene.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, if the examiner had access to suggestive information, the worksheet can be presented as evidence that adequate mitigation strategies were (or were not) taken to counteract the biasing effect of that information. Indeed, studies have found that jurors can use this type of information appropriately when evaluating forensic evidence: When alerted to an examiner's poor performance on proficiency tests [ 26 ] or the possibility that bias tainted the expert's judgment [ 27 ], mock jurors appropriately devalued the expert's testimony—and conversely, when informed that the expert followed context management procedures to reduce the risk of bias, mock jurors gave more weight to their testimony [ 28 ]. Thus, the worksheet can sensitize jurors in cases where skepticism is warranted, but also bolster the credibility of experts who use appropriate mitigation strategies.…”
Section: The Promise Of Information Management Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, fingerprint examiners compare latent fingerprints from crime scenes with exemplar fingerprints from suspects and opine as to whether the two prints match. These judgements are persuasive to fact‐finders in court (e.g., Crozier et al, 2020; Lieberman et al, 2008), yet leading scientific bodies have noted a dearth of research demonstrating the reliability and validity of these judgements (National Academy of Sciences, 2009; President's Council of Advisors on Science and Technology, 2016). Whilst such research is beginning to emerge (Towler et al, 2018), many existing studies are limited by their use of equal numbers of 'match' and 'non‐match' trials (e.g., Busey & Vanderkolk, 2005; Searston & Tangen, 2017; Towler et al, 2017; White et al, 2015).…”
Section: Ground Truth Match Non‐matchmentioning
confidence: 99%