2019
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-56380-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Kea (Nestor notabilis) represent object trajectory and identity

Abstract: The ability to represent both the identity and trajectory of hidden objects underlies our capacity to reason about causal mechanisms. However, to date no studies have shown that non-human animals are capable of representing these two factors simultaneously. Here, we tested whether kea can represent out-of-sight object trajectories and identities by presenting subjects with three tasks, each of which involved tracking or predicting hand trajectories as they moved behind a screen. Taken together, our results sug… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
(28 reference statements)
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This control sought to test whether kea exhibited any natural preferences for or against lid-popping, prior to experiencing any experimental trials. Given their experimental background and extent of daily environmental enrichment 33 , 34 , we needed to determine if they would engage in the task at all. Only two subjects showed a statistically significantly preference for the lidded cup (Neo and Plankton, see Table 2 ), but all eight subjects chose the lidded cup at least half the time (sign test, p = .016), indicating, at the group level, a preference for popping lids off the empty cups.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This control sought to test whether kea exhibited any natural preferences for or against lid-popping, prior to experiencing any experimental trials. Given their experimental background and extent of daily environmental enrichment 33 , 34 , we needed to determine if they would engage in the task at all. Only two subjects showed a statistically significantly preference for the lidded cup (Neo and Plankton, see Table 2 ), but all eight subjects chose the lidded cup at least half the time (sign test, p = .016), indicating, at the group level, a preference for popping lids off the empty cups.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous results suggest a full understanding of object permanence up to Stage 6 in several non-human species, including primates (e.g., Neiworth et al, 2003;Mendes and Huber, 2004; Barth and Call, 2006), corvids (Corvidae, e.g., Bugnyar et al, 2007;Hoffmann et al, 2011), psittacids (Psittacidae, e.g., Pepperberg et al, 1997;Auersperg et al, 2014;Bastos and Taylor, 2019), pigeons (Columba livia domestica, Zentall and Raley, 2019), and goats (Capra aegagrus hircus, Nawroth et al, 2015;Vas et al, 2019). Other species succeeded in tasks involving Stage 5, but not 6.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Where two cups are used, for example, they must reason that if the reward is not hidden in the cup shown to be empty, then it must be in the other one. Several species of corvids ( Schloegl et al, 2009 ; Mikolasch et al, 2012 ; Shaw et al, 2013 ; Jelbert et al, 2015 ), parrots ( Schloegl et al, 2009 ; Mikolasch et al, 2011 ; Pepperberg et al, 2013 ; O’Hara et al, 2015 , 2016 ; Bastos and Taylor, 2019 ; Subias et al, 2019 ), and apes ( Call, 2004 , 2006 ; Hill et al, 2011 ) readily reason in this way. The ability to reason by exclusion is present in some New World monkeys ( Sabbatini and Visalberghi, 2008 ; Marsh et al, 2015 ; Takahashi et al, 2015 ).…”
Section: Differences In Intelligence Across Speciesmentioning
confidence: 99%