1994
DOI: 10.1174/021435594321236082
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

La adquisición del lenguaje: talón de Aquiles y poción mágica de la teoría cognitiva <BR></BR>Language acquisition: Cognitive theory's Aquilles' heel and magic potion

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0
1

Year Published

1999
1999
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
2
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, certain grammatical properties have different activation and developmental schedules: some are acquired during childhood, while others are mastered later in school. For example, the expression of subjunctive mood in Spanish emerges by age 2 in monolingual children (López Ornat, 1994), but full command and use is not mastered until 12 years of age (Blake, 1983). When children are exposed to two languages, the amount of input in the home language is substantially reduced, and consequently, it is common to find signs of "attrition" in adulthood: structural properties that were acquired or mastered by age 4-6 are lost, resulting in difficulties with lexical production and interpretation (Schmid & Jarvis, 2014), or speakers use code-switching strategies and lexical borrowings to compensate for gaps during speech (J.…”
Section: Age and Linguistic Experience In L2 Acquisitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, certain grammatical properties have different activation and developmental schedules: some are acquired during childhood, while others are mastered later in school. For example, the expression of subjunctive mood in Spanish emerges by age 2 in monolingual children (López Ornat, 1994), but full command and use is not mastered until 12 years of age (Blake, 1983). When children are exposed to two languages, the amount of input in the home language is substantially reduced, and consequently, it is common to find signs of "attrition" in adulthood: structural properties that were acquired or mastered by age 4-6 are lost, resulting in difficulties with lexical production and interpretation (Schmid & Jarvis, 2014), or speakers use code-switching strategies and lexical borrowings to compensate for gaps during speech (J.…”
Section: Age and Linguistic Experience In L2 Acquisitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children use certain biases or innate cognitive mechanisms to learn words, but this learning always occurs within a context of interaction during which, due to the salient characteristics of the referents, children can detect the relationships between the discourse and the world, and in this way comprehend the speaker’s communicative intent. A good deal of studies on language development bring together innatist ideas with proposals centred around the importance of the ontogenetic experience in language acquisition (for example, Bavin, 2009; Karmiloff & Karmiloff-Smith, 2001; López Ornat, 1994, 2003; López Ornat & Gallo, 2004).…”
Section: Learning Lexiconmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Los niños se sirven de ciertos sesgos o mecanismos cognitivos innatos para aprender palabras, pero este aprendizaje se da en un contexto de interacción en el que, gracias a las características salientes de los referentes, los niños pueden detectar las relaciones entre el discurso y el mundo, y así comprender la intención comunicativa del hablante. Múltiples investigaciones en desarrollo del lenguaje reconcilian las ideas innatistas con las propuestas centradas en la importancia de la experiencia ontogenética en la adquisición del lenguaje (por ejemplo, Bavin, 2009; Karmiloff & Karmiloff-Smith, 2001; López Ornat, 1994, 2003; López Ornat & Gallo, 2004).…”
Section: Aprendiendo Un Adjetivo El Impacto De La Información Descrip...unclassified