PsycEXTRA Dataset 1972
DOI: 10.1037/e461882004-001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lack of bilateral generalization of adaptation to auditory rearrangement.

Abstract: Theoretical speculations concerning the presence or absence of intermanual transfer under differing conditions of sensorimotor adaptation have assumed that the adaptation produced by them are essentially equivalent. The possibility remains that sensorimotor changes generated by these different exposure conditions may be dissimilar. In order to resolve this, it is necessary to obtain relevant information from exposure conditions that provide adequate controls. Auditory rearrangement produced by functional rotat… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

1974
1974
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In most such studies with vision, the perceptual effects of viewing the world through prisms that rotate the visual field are investigated. In the majority of such studies with audition, the question of how mean localization judgments change when the interaural axis is effectively rotated is investigated (see, e.g., Day & Singer, 1967;Kalil & Freedman, 1967;Mikaelian, 1969Mikaelian, , 1972Mikaelian, , 1974Recanzone, 1998;Willey, Inglis, & Pearce, 1937;Young, 1928). (For a review of auditory adaptation, see ShinnCunningham, Lehnert, Kramer, Wenzel, & Durlach, 1997; for a review of adaptation studies in general, see Welch, 1978.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In most such studies with vision, the perceptual effects of viewing the world through prisms that rotate the visual field are investigated. In the majority of such studies with audition, the question of how mean localization judgments change when the interaural axis is effectively rotated is investigated (see, e.g., Day & Singer, 1967;Kalil & Freedman, 1967;Mikaelian, 1969Mikaelian, , 1972Mikaelian, , 1974Recanzone, 1998;Willey, Inglis, & Pearce, 1937;Young, 1928). (For a review of auditory adaptation, see ShinnCunningham, Lehnert, Kramer, Wenzel, & Durlach, 1997; for a review of adaptation studies in general, see Welch, 1978.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The results, obtained by Young (1928) and Willey et al (1937), that there were no auditory aftereffects when the reversing pseudophone was removed after their subjects had been exposed to the auditory rearrangement led the investigators to conclude that human subjects are incapable of reacquiring normal auditory localization of unseen sound sources when auditory space was pseudophonically reversed (Welch, 1978). Thus, later investigators have examined human adaptability to a " less drastic" auditory rearrangement (e. g., Freedman, Wilson, & Rekosh, 1967;Held, 1955;Mikaelian, 1972).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Additivity One of the most firmly established facts of prism adaptation is the additivity of the component aftereffects (e.g., Hay & Pick, 1966;Hay, 1970;Hay & Brouchon, 1972;Mikaelian, 1970Mikaelian, , 1972Mikaelian, , 1974Redding, 1978;Redding & Wallace, 1976, 1990Templeton et al, 1974;Wallace & Garrett, 1975;Wallace & Redding, 1979;Wilkinson, 1971). That is, visual and proprioceptive aftereffects usually sum to the total aftereffect in the eye-hand coordination loop exercised during exposure.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%