Pigeons and adult humans searched for a 2-cm 2 unmarked goal in digitized images of an outdoor scene presented on a touch-screen monitor. In Experiment 1, the scene contained three landmarks near the goal and a visually rich background. Six training images presented the scene from different viewing directions and distances. Subsequent unreinforced tests in which landmark or background cues were removed or shifted revealed that pigeons' search was controlled by both proximal landmarks and background cues, whereas humans relied only on the proximal landmarks. Pigeons' search accuracy dropped substantially when they were presented with novel views of the same scene, whereas humans showed perfect transfer to novel views. In Experiment 2, pigeons with previous outdoor experience and humans were trained with 28 views of an outdoor scene. Both pigeons and humans transferred well to novel views of the scene. This positive transfer suggests that, under some conditions, pigeons, like humans, may encode the three-dimensional spatial information in images of a scene.Many organisms have been shown to use visual landmarks to remember and locate a goal area (see Collett, 1992, andGallistel, 1990, for reviews). Use ofvisual landmarks is demonstrated experimentally by manipulating the visual cues between the opportunity to encode the spatial information and subsequent search tests in which the subject attempts to locate the goal. Two types of evidence indicate that a subject uses a particular landmark: (I) the subject shifts its search location in response to landmark shifts, and (2) accurate search is disrupted by the absence of the landmark. In a series of experiments using a laboratory task in which food is hidden on the floor of a spatial arena, Cheng and his colleagues (e.g., Cheng, 1988Cheng, , 1989Cheng, , 1994Cheng & Sherry, 1992) have clearly demonstrated that pigeons use visual landmarks to locate a hidden goal, and they have identified several principles of pigeons' landmark-based search.Pigeons' landmark-based search has also been investigated in a touch-screen task in which pigeons search for an unmarked goal on the surface of a monitor (Spetch, Cheng, & Mondloch, 1992;Spetch & Mondloch, 1993;Spetch & Wilkie, 1994
85search task has yielded results that are remarkably similar in certain ways to those found in real-space tasks. In particular, near landmarks are weighted more heavily than are far landmarks (Cheng, 1989;Spetch & Wilkie, 1994), and landmarks near an edge ofthe search space exert more control in the dimension parallel to the edge than in the dimension perpendicular to the edge (Cheng & Sherry, 1992; Spetch et aI., 1992). Recent work (Spetch, Cheng, & MacDonald, 1996; Spetch et aI., 1997) has revealed that, on both the touch screen and the laboratory floor, pigeons can use the configuration of an array of landmarks to locate a goal. In both environments, however, they respond to expansions of the array by maintaining the absolute training distance from individual landmarks in the array, rather than adju...