1981
DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(81)90097-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lateralized sensitivity to vibrotactile stimulation: Individual differences revealed by interaction of threshold and signal detection tasks

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Left-side superiority has also been reported for pressure, pain, sharpness, and vibrotactile thresholds (Ghent 1961;Haslam 1970;Rhodes and Schwartz 1981;Semmes et al 1960;Weinstein and Sersen 1961), though Carmon et al (1969), and Fennel et al (1967) were unable to demonstrate laterality differences. Boll (1974) found that patients with right-hemisphere damage were more impaired on a battery of tactualperception tasks than those with left-hemisphere damage and concluded that while the left hemisphere may subserve contralateral tactual perception, the right is preeminent for both sides of the body.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Left-side superiority has also been reported for pressure, pain, sharpness, and vibrotactile thresholds (Ghent 1961;Haslam 1970;Rhodes and Schwartz 1981;Semmes et al 1960;Weinstein and Sersen 1961), though Carmon et al (1969), and Fennel et al (1967) were unable to demonstrate laterality differences. Boll (1974) found that patients with right-hemisphere damage were more impaired on a battery of tactualperception tasks than those with left-hemisphere damage and concluded that while the left hemisphere may subserve contralateral tactual perception, the right is preeminent for both sides of the body.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Participants sat in a light-attenuated room approximately 60 cm in front of a stimulus array, consisting of a polystyrene wedge in which was mounted a 4 mm red light-emitting diode (LED) and a bone conductor with a 1.6 cm62.4 cm vibrating surface (Oticon Limited, B/C 2-PIN), to which the participant's index finger was fixed with a double-sided adhesive pad. Square-wave (100 Hz) stimuli lasting 20 ms were presented to the left index finger, in light of evidence that the left (nondominant) hand is more sensitive to vibrotactile and somatosensory stimuli than the right (dominant) hand (eg Ghent 1961;Goldblatt 1956;Rhodes and Schwartz 1981).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies involving psychophysics and neuroimaging have also demonstrated a right-hemisphere advantage for haptic processing in both humans ( De Renzi et al, 1969 ; Milner and Taylor, 1972 ; Benton et al, 1973 ; Dodds, 1978 ; Riege et al, 1980 ; O’Boyle et al, 1987 ; Wilkinson and Carr, 1987 ; Fagot et al, 1993a , b , 1994 ; Butler et al, 2004 ; Harada et al, 2004 ; Loayza et al, 2011 ; Morange-Majoux, 2011 ; Tomlinson et al, 2011 ; Cormier and Tremblay, 2013 ; Stone and Gonzalez, 2014a , b ) and non-human primates ( Lacreuse and Fragaszy, 1996 , 1999 ). For most of these studies, individuals have been asked to haptically explore, differentiate, or detect geometrical shapes ( Franco and Sperry, 1977 ; Cormier and Tremblay, 2013 ; Stone and Gonzalez, 2014a , b ), non-sense shapes ( Dodds, 1978 ; Fagot et al, 1993a , b , 1994 ), vibrations ( Weinstein, 1978 ; Rhodes and Schwartz, 1981 ; Heller et al, 1990 ; Wiles et al, 1990 ), or object orientation ( Cannon and Benton, 1969 ; Benton et al, 1973 ; Varney and Benton, 1975 ; Brizzolara et al, 1982 ). For instance, Fagot et al (1993a , b , 1994 ) had individuals haptically explore different cubes either unimanually or bimanually and measured accuracy during a recognition test.…”
Section: Sensory Contributions To Hand Preference For Reaching and Grmentioning
confidence: 99%