2016
DOI: 10.1353/lan.2016.0071
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learnability Shapes Typology: The Case of the Midpoint Pathology

Abstract: The midpoint pathology (in the sense of Kager 2012) characterizes a type of unattested stress system in which the stressable window contracts to a single word-internal syllable in some words, but not others. Kager (2012) shows that the pathology is a prediction of analyses employing contextual lapse constraints (e.g. *ExtLapseR; no 000 strings at the right edge) and argues that the only way to avoid it is to eliminate these constraints from Con. This article explores an alternative: that systems exhibiting the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
23
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
1
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Typological underrepresentation may well be strongly related to reduced learnability. Second, the non-directional character of unattested BTU-patterns is usually only apparent in forms of considerable length, which again reduces the learnability of the non-directional pattern, given reasonable assumptions about the limited amount of relevant input to the learner in the form of long words (Stanton 2014). Consider, for example, the third BTU-pattern in category F, which is identical to the first and second ones for forms with fewer than six syllables.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Typological underrepresentation may well be strongly related to reduced learnability. Second, the non-directional character of unattested BTU-patterns is usually only apparent in forms of considerable length, which again reduces the learnability of the non-directional pattern, given reasonable assumptions about the limited amount of relevant input to the learner in the form of long words (Stanton 2014). Consider, for example, the third BTU-pattern in category F, which is identical to the first and second ones for forms with fewer than six syllables.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…discussion of word-order typology in Bickel 2007: 241), language-specific historical contingencies (Nichols 1992, Blevins 2004, Hansson 2008, Harris 2008 and differential learnability (e.g. Hayes & Wilson 2008, Culbertson 2012, Moreton & Pater 2012, Stanton 2016 17 as productive alternatives to innate constraints. We independently need explanations and theories of each of these domains, and each is a strictly simpler type of explanation of typological commonality and rarity than the non-specific, catch-all assumption of innate knowledge or constraints.…”
Section: How Should We Establish and Explain Typological Generalisatimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Staubs [25,26] shows that learning biases explain the limited size of stress windows, obviating the need for the explicit stating of such size limits in grammar theory. Similarly, Stanton [27] shows that learning considerations can provide an alternative account for the typological finding that no language places stress on the middle positions of a stress domain. Specifically, learning such middle-stress patterns is complicated by the fact that exposure to longer words is necessary to disambiguate the pattern from edge-based patterns, and that the used learning algorithm extracts conflicting information from the various data types it is exposed to.…”
Section: Simulated Learning 41 Learning and Typologymentioning
confidence: 99%