The Cambridge Handbook of Learner Corpus Research 2015
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9781139649414.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learner corpora and phraseology

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As mentioned above, one of the major differences between L1 and L2 collocational production was related to the proportion of collocations with high MI‐score values (the values were established on the basis of reference corpora such as the BNC). Compared to L1 users, collocations found in L2 production were more likely to appear frequently in the reference corpora and were less strongly associated on the MI‐score; by contrast, L1 users produced more collocations with higher MI‐score values (e.g., Ebeling & Hasselgård, ; Granger & Bestgen, ). As discussed in the section “MI‐score,” the strength in this AM is a result of the interaction of exclusivity and the low frequency of the word co‐occurrence.…”
Section: Interpreting the Evidence: Collocational Usage Patterns In Lmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…As mentioned above, one of the major differences between L1 and L2 collocational production was related to the proportion of collocations with high MI‐score values (the values were established on the basis of reference corpora such as the BNC). Compared to L1 users, collocations found in L2 production were more likely to appear frequently in the reference corpora and were less strongly associated on the MI‐score; by contrast, L1 users produced more collocations with higher MI‐score values (e.g., Ebeling & Hasselgård, ; Granger & Bestgen, ). As discussed in the section “MI‐score,” the strength in this AM is a result of the interaction of exclusivity and the low frequency of the word co‐occurrence.…”
Section: Interpreting the Evidence: Collocational Usage Patterns In Lmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We selected three types of collocations representing a range of constructions that commonly appear in collocational research (Durrant & Schmitt, ; Ebeling & Hasselgård, ; Granger & Bestgen, ; Paquot & Granger, ; Siyanova & Schmitt, ): verb + complementation ( make + sure/decision/point ), adjective + noun ( human + beings/rights/nature ), and adverb + adjective ( vitally/very/really + important ). Due to space constraints, only the results for the first construction, that is, verb + complementation, are presented in Table ; results for the other two structures, which echo the points made here, can be found in Appendix S3 in the Supporting Information online.…”
Section: Comparing Collocations Across Different Linguistic Settings:mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The corpus‐based association measures used in psycholinguistic studies are likely to directly and significantly affect the findings and, consequently, the insights into language learning and processing (Gablasova et al., 2017) derived through them. Although various studies with a corpus linguistic focus have made efforts to standardize the conflicting terminology (e.g., Ebeling & Hasselgård, 2015; Evert, 2008; Gablasova et al., 2017), the rationales behind the selection of the association measures in psycholinguistic studies have not always been fully transparent and systematic. Despite the availability of many association measures (see comprehensive overviews by Evert, 2005; Pecina, 2009; Wiechmann, 2008), so far, the mutual information measure has been predominantly used in psycholinguistic research either to extract collocations (e.g., Vilkaité, 2016) or to investigate language users’ sensitivity to collocational strength (e.g., Yi, 2018).…”
Section: Background Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%