2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.10.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning, awareness, and instruction: Subjective contingency awareness does matter in the colour-word contingency learning paradigm

Abstract: In three experiments, each of a set colour-unrelated distracting words was presented most often in a particular target print colour (e.g., "month" most often in red). In Experiment 1, half of the participants were told the word-colour contingencies in advance (instructed) and half were not (control). The instructed group showed a larger learning effect. This instruction effect was fully explained by increases in subjective awareness with instruction. In Experiment 2, contingency instructions were again given, … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

9
45
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
9
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Interestingly, participants generally have very low levels of contingency awareness in this task (Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012a, 2012b, 2012c, 2012d, similar to other tasks (e.g., Destrebecqz & Cleeremans, 2001;Lewicki, 1985;McKelvie, 1987;Miller, 1987).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 80%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Interestingly, participants generally have very low levels of contingency awareness in this task (Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012a, 2012b, 2012c, 2012d, similar to other tasks (e.g., Destrebecqz & Cleeremans, 2001;Lewicki, 1985;McKelvie, 1987;Miller, 1987).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…We then compared high, medium, and low contingency trials. Unsurprisingly, high contingency trials were responded to significantly suggest that contingencies are learned quickly and remain relatively stable throughout the task (Schmidt et al, 2007(Schmidt et al, , 2010Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012b, 2012d. conditions were small (<0.5%) and in the opposite direction to those predicted by the misprediction cost and bidirectional cost accounts.…”
Section: (Figure 3)mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Each word was presented most often in a certain colour (e.g., "rent" most often in purple), making for high contingency items (rent purple ) and low contingency items (rent orange ). Despite a general lack of awareness of the manipulation, participants are known to respond faster and more accurately to high contingency items (Schmidt et al, 2007;Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012a, 2012b, 2012c, 2012dSchmidt, De Houwer, & Besner, 2010). Thus, high contingency trials are fast, and low contingency or non-contingency trials are slow.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to practice curves, episodic memory models have been proposed for a broad range of other performance phenomena, including contingency learning (Schmidt, Crump, Cheesman, & Besner, 2007;Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012a, 2012c, 2012d, 2016aSchmidt, De Houwer, & Besner, 2010), feature binding effects (Frings, Rothermund, & Wentura, 2007;Hommel, 1998), negative priming (Rothermund, Wentura, & De Houwer, 2005), proportion congruent effects (Schmidt, 2013a(Schmidt, , 2013b, congruency sequence effects (Hommel et al, 2004;Mayr, Awh, & Laurey, 2003;Mordkoff, 2012;Schmidt & De Houwer, 2011), evaluative conditioning (Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012b), task switch costs (Logan & Bundesen, 2003Logan & Schneider, 2006a, 2006bLogan, Schneider, & Bundesen, 2007;Schmidt & Liefooghe, 2016;, rhythmic responding (Kinoshita et al, 2008;Kinoshita et al, 2011;Mozer et al, 2004;Schmidt, 2013cSchmidt, , 2014Schmidt, Lemercier, et al, 2014;Schmidt & Weissman, 2016), and various other phenomena.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%